Cue-Driven Changes in Detection Strategies Reflect Trade-Offs in Strategic Efficiency

We examined performance in a cued detection task when a peripheral cue was either 50% or 100% valid, tested in separate experiments. We combined a Posner cueing task with a double factorial manipulation of stimulus salience. Unlike previous investigations in which participants responded to either target, we employed an AND decision task in which a target-present decision required there to be a target at both locations. When the cue was 50% valid, all the participants adopted parallel exhaustive processing to detect redundant targets with unlimited to limited capacity. When the cue was 100% valid, three participants, who performed this experiment first, adopted serial exhaustive processing. By contrast, the participants who first performed the 50% validity experiment continued to adopt parallel exhaustive processing. Capacity generally declined below a lower bound, suggesting extremely limited capacity. Our conclusion is that the validity of the cue affected processing strategy but participants could increase the relative efficiency of the parallel processing with practice.

[1]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[2]  D. Raab DIVISION OF PSYCHOLOGY: STATISTICAL FACILITATION OF SIMPLE REACTION TIMES* , 1962 .

[3]  W. Levick,et al.  Sustained and transient neurones in the cat's retina and lateral geniculate nucleus , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[5]  D. A. Taylor Effect of identity in the multiletter matching task. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  R. Luce,et al.  Evidence from auditory simple reaction times for both change and level detectors , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[11]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of practice in fact retrieval. , 1985 .

[12]  N. Perrin,et al.  Varieties of perceptual independence. , 1986, Psychological review.

[13]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[14]  H Colonius,et al.  Measuring channel dependence in separate activation models , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  A. Lambert,et al.  Automaticity and the capture of attention by a peripheral display change , 1987 .

[16]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[17]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[18]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption , 1989 .

[19]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention , 1989, Vision Research.

[20]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  H. Colonius Possibly dependent probability summation of reaction time , 1990 .

[22]  J. Townsend Serial vs. Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and Should) be Distinguished , 1990 .

[23]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  M. Cheal,et al.  Central and Peripheral Precuing of Forced-Choice Discrimination , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  Jeff Miller Channel interaction and the redundant-targets effect in bimodal divided attention. , 1991 .

[26]  G. Logan Shapes of reaction-time distributions and shapes of learning curves: a test of the instance theory of automaticity. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Relations between prototype, exemplar, and decision bound models of categorization , 1993 .

[28]  D. Vorberg,et al.  Distribution inequalities for parallel models with unlimited capacity , 1994 .

[29]  S. Yantis,et al.  Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  J. Townsend,et al.  Stochastic dependencies in parallel and serial models: effects on systems factorial interactions , 1994 .

[31]  F. Ashby,et al.  Categorization as probability density estimation , 1995 .

[32]  Philip L. Smith Psychophysically principled models of visual simple reaction time. , 1995 .

[33]  J. Townsend,et al.  Spatio-temporal properties of elementary perception: an investigation of parallel, serial, and coactive theories , 1995 .

[34]  Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997 .

[35]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[37]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[38]  Dzhafarov Conditionally Selective Dependence of Random Variables on External Factors. , 1999, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[39]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Effects of similarity and practice on speeded classification response times and accuracies: Further tests of an exemplar-retrieval model , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[40]  D. Madden,et al.  Age differences in the strategic allocation of visual attention. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[41]  Robert L. Goldstone Unitization during category learning. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Robert L. Goldstone Unitization during category learning , 2000 .

[43]  T. Zandt,et al.  How to fit a response time distribution , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  E. Dzhafarov Unconditionally Selective Dependence of Random Variables on External Factors. , 2001, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[45]  B McElree,et al.  Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Quantile maximum likelihood estimation of response time distributions , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[47]  J. Townsend,et al.  A theory of interactive parallel processing: new capacity measures and predictions for a response time inequality series. , 2004, Psychological review.

[48]  J. Dunn Remember-know: a matter of confidence. , 2004, Psychological review.

[49]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Aging, practice, and perceptual tasks: a diffusion model analysis. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[50]  J. Kujala,et al.  Testing for selectivity in the dependence of random variables on external factors , 2008 .

[51]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Information-processing architectures in multidimensional classification: a validation test of the systems factorial technology. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Orienting of Attention , 2008 .

[53]  M. Carrasco,et al.  On the automaticity and flexibility of covert attention: a speed-accuracy trade-off analysis. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[54]  J. Townsend,et al.  An LDA Approach to the Neural Correlates of Configural Learning , 2009 .

[55]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  A diffusion model decomposition of the practice effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Distribution-free tests of stochastic dominance for small samples , 2010 .

[57]  Aaron B. Hoffman,et al.  The costs of supervised classification: The effect of learning task on conceptual flexibility. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Logical-rule models of classification response times: a synthesis of mental-architecture, random-walk, and decision-bound approaches. , 2010, Psychological review.

[59]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The statistical properties of the Survivor Interaction Contrast , 2010 .

[60]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[61]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  Nice Guys Finish Fast and Bad Guys Finish Last: Facilitatory vs. Inhibitory Interaction in Parallel Systems. , 2011, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[62]  J. Townsend,et al.  Workload capacity spaces: A unified methodology for response time measures of efficiency as workload is varied , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[63]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Short-term memory scanning viewed as exemplar-based categorization. , 2011, Psychological review.

[64]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Task-related versus stimulus-specific practice. , 2011, Experimental psychology.

[65]  David K. Sewell,et al.  Attentional control in visual signal detection: effects of abrupt-onset and no-onset stimuli. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[66]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  The structure of short-term memory scanning: an investigation using response time distribution models , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[67]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  Accounting for speed–accuracy tradeoff in perceptual learning , 2012, Vision Research.

[68]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Logical rules and the classification of integral-dimension stimuli. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[69]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  Systems factorial technology with R , 2014, Behavior research methods.

[70]  Cheng-Ta Yang,et al.  The influence of cueing on attentional focus in perceptual decision making , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[71]  J. Dana,et al.  Comparing the accuracy of experimental estimates to guessing: a new perspective on replication and the “Crisis of Confidence” in psychology , 2013, Behavior Research Methods.

[72]  Anthea G. Blunden,et al.  Logical-rules and the classification of integral dimensions: individual differences in the processing of arbitrary dimensions , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[73]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Identifying strategy use in category learning tasks: a case for more diagnostic data and models. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[74]  David K. Sewell,et al.  The Psychology and Psychobiology of Simple Decisions: Speeded Choice and Its Neural Correlates , 2016 .

[75]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  The perceptual processing of fused multi-spectral imagery , 2016, Cognitive research: principles and implications.

[76]  Robert Taylor,et al.  Resources masquerading as slots: Flexible allocation of visual working memory , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.

[77]  Cheng-Ta Yang,et al.  An examination of parallel versus coactive processing accounts of redundant-target audiovisual signal processing , 2017 .

[78]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Historical Foundations and a Tutorial Introduction to Systems Factorial Technology , 2017 .

[79]  L. Blaha An Examination of Task Demands on the Elicited Processing Capacity , 2017 .

[80]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Systems Factorial Technology: A Theory Driven Methodology for the Identification of Perceptual and Cognitive Mechanisms , 2017 .

[81]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Stretching Mental Processes: An Overview of and Guide for SFT Applications , 2017 .

[82]  Cheng-Ta Yang,et al.  Set size slope still does not distinguish parallel from serial search. , 2017, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[83]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Activation-state representation of models for the redundant-signals-effect. , 1997 .