Differences in Manufacturing Traditions and Assemblage-Level Patterns: the Origins of Cultural Differences in Archaeological Data

A relationship between behavioral variability and artifactual variability is a founding principle of archaeology. However, this relationship is surprisingly not well studied empirically from an explicitly “microevolutionary” perspective. Here, we experimentally simulated artifactual variation in two populations of “artifact” manufacturers, involving only a single behavioral difference in terms of their “tradition” of manufacturing tool. We then statistically analyzed shape variation in the resultant artifacts. In many respects, patterned differences might not have been expected to emerge given the simple nature of the task, the fact that only a single behavioral variable differed in our two populations, and all participants copied the same target artifact. However, multivariate analyses identified significant differences between the two “assemblages.” These results have several implications for our understanding and theoretical conceptualization of the relationship between behavior and artifactual variability, including the analytical potency of conceiving of artifacts as the product of behavioral “recipes” comprised of individual “ingredient” behavioral properties. Indeed, quite trivial behavioral differences, in generating microevolutionarily potent variability, can thus have long-term consequences for artifactual changes measured over time and space. Moreover, measurable “cultural” differences in artifacts can emerge not necessarily only because of a strict “mental template” but as the result of subtle differences in behavioral ingredients that are socially learned at the community level.

[1]  Peter C. Jordan Technology as Human Social Tradition: Cultural Transmission among Hunter-Gatherers , 2014 .

[2]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Quantifying Transitions: Morphometric Approaches to Palaeolithic Variability and Technological Change , 2009 .

[3]  Prudence M. Rice,et al.  Pottery Analysis: A Source Book , 1989 .

[4]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  An Experimental Test of the Accumulated Copying Error Model of Cultural Mutation for Acheulean Handaxe Size , 2012, PloS one.

[5]  M. Hamilton,et al.  A Formal Test of the Origin of Variation in North American Early Paleoindian Projectile Points , 2009, American Antiquity.

[6]  Diane F. Halpern,et al.  Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities. Third Edition. , 2000 .

[7]  Todd L. VanPool,et al.  The diversity of North American projectile-point types, before and after the bow and arrow , 2009 .

[8]  M. J. O’Brien,et al.  Social learning and technological evolution during the Clovis colonization of the New World. , 2015, Journal of human evolution.

[9]  Jelmer W. Eerkens,et al.  Practice Makes Within 5% of Perfect: Visual Perception, Motor Skills, and Memory in Artifact Variation1 , 2000, Current Anthropology.

[10]  Ashley M. Smallwood Clovis Technology and Settlement in the American Southeast: Using Biface Analysis to Evaluate Dispersal Models , 2012, American Antiquity.

[11]  C. Heyes,et al.  SOCIAL LEARNING IN ANIMALS: CATEGORIES AND MECHANISMS , 1994, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[12]  P. Sniegowski,et al.  As it happens: current directions in experimental evolution , 2013, Biology Letters.

[13]  P. Rice,et al.  Pottery Analysis: A Sourcebook , 1987 .

[14]  Diane F. Halpern,et al.  Sex differences in cognitive abilities, 2nd ed. , 1992 .

[15]  Peter Bleed,et al.  The Optimal Design of Hunting Weapons: Maintainability or Reliability , 1986, American Antiquity.

[16]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Acheulean variation and selection: does handaxe symmetry fit neutral expectations? , 2008 .

[17]  T. Garland,et al.  Experimental Evolution: Concepts, Methods, and Applications of Selection Experiments , 2009 .

[18]  James M. Skibo,et al.  The Explanation of Artifact Variability , 1997, American Antiquity.

[19]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Descent with modification and the archaeological record , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  S. Lycett,et al.  Toward a “Quantitative Genetic” Approach to Lithic Variation , 2015 .

[21]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  Factors affecting Acheulean handaxe variation: Experimental insights, microevolutionary processes, and macroevolutionary outcomes , 2016 .

[22]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  A crossbeam co-ordinate caliper for the morphometric analysis of lithic nuclei: a description, test and empirical examples of application , 2006 .

[23]  M. J. O’Brien,et al.  Applying Evolutionary Archaeology: A Systematic Approach , 2000 .

[24]  M. Robert,et al.  Does men’s advantage in mental rotation persist when real three-dimensional objects are either felt or seen? , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[25]  J. Deetz,et al.  Invitation to archaeology , 1967 .

[26]  Carl P. Lipo,et al.  Cultural Transmission Theory and the Archaeological Record: Providing Context to Understanding Variation and Temporal Changes in Material Culture , 2007 .

[27]  W. McGrew The Cultured Chimpanzee: Contents , 2004 .

[28]  Marcus W Feldman,et al.  Inferring population histories using cultural data , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  G. London The Clay Sleeps: An Ethnoarchaeological Study of Three African Potters , 1987, American Antiquity.

[30]  Michael J. O'Brien,et al.  Cultural traits as units of analysis , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  David J. Meltzer,et al.  A Study of Style and Function in a Class of Tools , 1981 .

[32]  M. J. O’Brien,et al.  Evolutionary implications of design and performance characteristics of prehistoric pottery , 1994 .

[33]  Behavioral Archaeology: Four Strategies1 , 1975 .

[34]  W. Wendrich The World According to Basketry , 1999 .

[35]  Thomas Wynn,et al.  Evolution of sex differences in spatial cognition , 1996 .

[36]  C. Orton,et al.  Pottery in archaeology , 1995 .

[37]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  The Importance of a “Quantitative Genetic” Approach to the Evolution of Artifact Morphological Traits , 2016 .

[38]  Briggs Buchanan,et al.  The accumulation of stochastic copying errors causes drift in culturally transmitted technologies: Quantifying Clovis evolutionary dynamics , 2009 .

[39]  O. W. Hampton Culture of Stone: Sacred and Profane Uses of Stone among the Dani , 1999 .

[40]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  The impact of imitative versus emulative learning mechanisms on artifactual variation: implications for the evolution of material culture , 2015 .

[41]  J. Gowlett The Future of Lithic Analysis in Palaeolithic Archaeology: A View from the Old World , 2010 .

[42]  Michael B. Schiffer,et al.  Theory and Experiment in the Study of Technological Change , 1987, Current Anthropology.

[43]  R. Greenspan Fly pushing : the theory and practice of Drosophila genetics , 1996 .

[44]  W. Jungers,et al.  Shape, relative size, and size‐adjustments in morphometrics , 1995 .

[45]  M. Ghiselin The triumph of the Darwinian method , 1970 .

[46]  M. Ross The Genetics and Biology of Drosophila, Vol. 1B , 1976 .

[47]  W. Jungers,et al.  Morphometrics of the callitrichid forelimb: A case study in size and shape , 1993, International Journal of Primatology.

[48]  Marius Kempe,et al.  Learning in the Acheulean: Experimental Insights Using Handaxe Form as a ‘Model Organism’ , 2015 .

[49]  Tobias Bachmeier The Cultured Chimpanzee Reflections On Cultural Primatology , 2016 .

[50]  H. L. Carson,et al.  The Genetics and Biology of Drosophila , 1976, Heredity.

[51]  D. Halpern Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities , 1986 .

[52]  David Jones,et al.  Size and shape , 1996, Nature.

[53]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  Copying Error and the Cultural Evolution of “Additive” vs. “Reductive” Material Traditions: An Experimental Assessment , 2014, American Antiquity.

[54]  Patricia Davison,et al.  The Clay Sleeps: An Ethno-Archaeological Study of Three African Potters. , 1986 .

[55]  Frederick R. Matson,et al.  Ceramics for the Archaeologist , 1968, American Antiquity.

[56]  Susan D. Voyer,et al.  Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  J. Gowlett Form 2 The elements of design form in Acheulian bifaces : modes , modalities , rules and language , 2005 .

[58]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Population, Culture History, and the Dynamics of Culture Change1 , 2000, Current Anthropology.

[59]  F. Neiman Stylistic Variation in Evolutionary Perspective: Inferences from Decorative Diversity and Interassemblage Distance in Illinois Woodland Ceramic Assemblages , 1995, American Antiquity.

[60]  L. Mueller 9. FITNESS, DEMOGRAPHY, AND POPULATION DYNAMICS IN LABORATORY EXPERIMENTS , 2019 .

[61]  Michael Ashburner,et al.  Drosophila: A laboratory handbook , 1990 .

[62]  Ø. Hammer,et al.  PAST: PALEONTOLOGICAL STATISTICAL SOFTWARE PACKAGE FOR EDUCATION AND DATA ANALYSIS , 2001 .

[63]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Cultural evolutionary approaches to artifact variation over time and space: basis, progress, and prospects , 2015 .

[64]  A. F. Bennett,et al.  2. THE IMPORTANCE OF EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES IN EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY , 2019 .

[65]  Michael J. O'Brien,et al.  The Cultural Transmission of Great Basin Projectile-Point Technology I: An Experimental Simulation , 2008, American Antiquity.

[66]  A. Mesoudi Cultural Evolution , 2011, eLS.

[67]  F. Bookstein “Size and Shape”: A Comment on Semantics , 1989 .

[68]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  Considering the Role of Time Budgets on Copy-Error Rates in Material Culture Traditions: An Experimental Assessment , 2014, PloS one.

[69]  Carl P. Lipo,et al.  Cultural transmission, copying errors, and the generation of variation in material culture and the archaeological record , 2005 .