The Differing Impact of Multisensory and Unisensory Integration on Behavior

Pooling and synthesizing signals across different senses often enhances responses to the event from which they are derived. Here, we examine whether multisensory response enhancements are attributable to a redundant target effect (two stimuli rather than one) or if there is some special quality inherent in the combination of cues from different senses. To test these possibilities, the performance of animals in localizing and detecting spatiotemporally concordant visual and auditory stimuli was examined when these stimuli were presented individually (visual or auditory) or in cross-modal (visual–auditory) and within-modal (visual–visual, auditory–auditory) combinations. Performance enhancements proved to be far greater for combinations of cross-modal than within-modal stimuli and support the idea that the behavioral products derived from multisensory integration are not attributable to simple target redundancy. One likely explanation is that whereas cross-modal signals offer statistically independent samples of the environment, within-modal signals can exhibit substantial covariance, and consequently multisensory integration can yield more substantial error reduction than unisensory integration.

[1]  W. Jiang,et al.  Two cortical areas mediate multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Y. Sakurai,et al.  Behavioral studies of auditory-visual spatial recognition and integration in rats , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Oscillatory beta activity predicts response speed during a multisensory audiovisual reaction time task: a high-density electrical mapping study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  A. Diederich,et al.  Bimodal and trimodal multisensory enhancement: Effects of stimulus onset and intensity on reaction time , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Miriam Reiner,et al.  Multisensory enhancement: gains in choice and in simple response times , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Michael T. Lippert,et al.  Improvement of visual contrast detection by a simultaneous sound , 2007, Brain Research.

[7]  M. Wallace,et al.  Superior colliculus neurons use distinct operational modes in the integration of multisensory stimuli. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  M. Wallace,et al.  Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Implicit redundant-targets effect in visual extinction , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  C. Spence,et al.  The co-occurrence of multisensory competition and facilitation. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[11]  J. Driver,et al.  Multisensory Interplay Reveals Crossmodal Influences on ‘Sensory-Specific’ Brain Regions, Neural Responses, and Judgments , 2008, Neuron.

[12]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[13]  Michael C. Dorris,et al.  Combined eye-head gaze shifts to visual and auditory targets in humans , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Fabrizio Leo,et al.  Multisensory integration for orienting responses in humans requires the activation of the superior colliculus , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Barry Stein,et al.  A Bayesian model unifies multisensory spatial localization with the physiological properties of the superior colliculus , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Visual-auditory interactions in sensorimotor processing: saccades versus manual responses. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Multisensory processing in the redundant-target effect: A behavioral and event-related potential study , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Cristiana Cavina-Pratesi,et al.  Redundant target effect and intersensory facilitation from visual-tactile interactions in simple reaction time , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  T. Stanford,et al.  Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  Silvia Savazzi,et al.  The superior colliculus subserves interhemispheric neural summation in both normals and patients with a total section or agenesis of the corpus callosum , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  B. Stein,et al.  The role of anterior ectosylvian cortex in cross-modality orientation and approach behavior , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Effects of Spatial Congruity on Audio-Visual Multimodal Integration , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Wallace,et al.  Representation and integration of multiple sensory inputs in primate superior colliculus. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  M. Alex Meredith,et al.  Neurons and behavior: the same rules of multisensory integration apply , 1988, Brain Research.

[26]  Silvia Savazzi,et al.  Speeding Up Reaction Time with Invisible Stimuli , 2002, Current Biology.

[27]  B. Stein,et al.  Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Miriam Reiner,et al.  Enhancement of response times to bi- and tri-modal sensory stimuli during active movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Visuo-spatial neural response interactions in early cortical processing during a simple reaction time task: a high-density electrical mapping study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  M. Frens,et al.  Spatial and temporal factors determine auditory-visual interactions in human saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  B. Stein,et al.  Two Corticotectal Areas Facilitate Multisensory Orientation Behavior , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Terrence R Stanford,et al.  A Model of the Neural Mechanisms Underlying Multisensory Integration in the Superior Colliculus , 2007, Perception.

[33]  B. Stein,et al.  Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. , 1983, Science.

[34]  T. Stanford,et al.  Superadditivity in multisensory integration: putting the computation in context. , 2007, Neuroreport.

[35]  Lawrence G. McDade,et al.  Behavioral Indices of Multisensory Integration: Orientation to Visual Cues is Affected by Auditory Stimuli , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  T. Stanford,et al.  Evaluating the Operations Underlying Multisensory Integration in the Cat Superior Colliculus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Sidney S. Simon,et al.  Merging of the Senses , 2008, Front. Neurosci..

[38]  M. Wallace,et al.  Superior colliculus lesions preferentially disrupt multisensory orientation , 2004, Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Giard,et al.  Auditory-Visual Integration during Multimodal Object Recognition in Humans: A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Effects of redundant auditory stimuli on reaction time , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  C. Spence,et al.  Multimodal visual–somatosensory integration in saccade generation , 2003, Neuropsychologia.