Visual Control of Locomotion
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Michaels,et al. To Cross or Not to Cross: The Effect of Locomotion on Street-Crossing Behavior , 1996 .
[2] Michael T. Turvey,et al. Perception, Categories, and Possibilities for Action , 2003, Adapt. Behav..
[3] Brett R. Fajen,et al. A Neural Model of MST and MT Explains Perceived Object Motion during Self-Motion , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] Michael T. Turvey,et al. Eye Movements and a Rule for Perceiving Direction of Heading , 1999 .
[5] W. Warren. Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action , 1988 .
[6] W. Warren,et al. Perception of translational heading from optical flow. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] Reinoud J Bootsma. The (current) future is here! , 2009, Perception.
[8] J. Wann,et al. Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[9] M. Lenoir,et al. Intercepting Moving Objects During Self-Motion. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[10] Jeff Punch,et al. A visualization of the flow and heat transfer from an oblique impinging jet generated in a square miniature channel , 2016, J. Vis..
[11] Brett R. Fajen. The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking. , 2005 .
[12] Marion A. Eppler,et al. Development of Visually Guided Locomotion , 1998 .
[13] Eli Brenner,et al. Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.
[14] David N. Lee,et al. A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.
[15] William H. Warren,et al. Chapter 8 – Self-Motion: Visual Perception and Visual Control , 1995 .
[16] G. DeAngelis,et al. Visual and vestibular cue integration for heading perception in extrastriate visual cortex , 2011, The Journal of physiology.
[17] Li Li,et al. Perceiving path from optic flow. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[18] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Internal Models in Biological Control , 2019, Annu. Rev. Control. Robotics Auton. Syst..
[19] J. Loomis,et al. Model-based control of perception/action , 2004 .
[20] Brett R Fajen,et al. Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] Dora E Angelaki,et al. Multisensory Integration of Visual and Vestibular Signals Improves Heading Discrimination in the Presence of a Moving Object , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[22] John A Perrone,et al. Visual-vestibular estimation of the body's curvilinear motion through the world: A computational model. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[23] Gavan Lintern,et al. Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997, Complex.
[24] Michael H Cole,et al. A systematic review on perceptual-motor calibration to changes in action capabilities. , 2017, Human movement science.
[25] Dora E Angelaki,et al. Causal inference accounts for heading perception in the presence of object motion , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] James A. Balliett,et al. What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. , 1990 .
[27] Dees Postma. Affordance-based control in running to catch fly balls , 2019 .
[28] W H Warren,et al. Perceiving Heading in the Presence of Moving Objects , 1995, Perception.
[29] R. Olberg,et al. Prey pursuit and interception in dragonflies , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[30] Constance S. Royden,et al. Analysis of misperceived observer motion during simulated eye rotations , 1994, Vision Research.
[31] J. Loomis,et al. Visually Controlled Locomotion: Its Dependence on Optic Flow, Three-Dimensional Space Perception, and Cognition , 1998 .
[32] Constance S. Royden,et al. Human heading judgments in the presence of moving objects , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] J. Konczak,et al. Changing affordances in stair climbing: the perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Constantin A Rothkopf,et al. The visual control of interceptive steering: How do people steer a car to intercept a moving target? , 2019, Journal of vision.
[35] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003 .
[36] James A. Crowell,et al. The perception of heading during eye movements , 1992, Nature.
[37] M. Turvey. Action and perception at the level of synergies. , 2007, Human movement science.
[38] H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al. The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[39] Julie M. Harris,et al. Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.
[40] K. J. Craik,et al. The nature of explanation , 1944 .
[41] W H Warren,et al. Visual control of braking: a test of the tau hypothesis. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] Frank T J M Zaal,et al. The Affordance of Catchability in Running to Intercept Fly Balls , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] Li Li,et al. Effects of reference objects and extra-retinal information about pursuit eye movements on curvilinear path perception from retinal flow. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[44] Matt J. Dunn,et al. Lateral visual occlusion does not change walking trajectories , 2018, Journal of vision.
[45] Cynthia F Moss,et al. Echolocating Bats Use a Nearly Time-Optimal Strategy to Intercept Prey , 2006, PLoS biology.
[46] Guy Wallis,et al. An Unexpected Role for Visual Feedback in Vehicle Steering Control , 2002, Current Biology.
[47] W. H. Warren. The dynamics of perception and action. , 2006, Psychological review.
[48] Brett R Fajen,et al. Visual Guidance of Intercepting a Moving Target on Foot , 2004, Perception.
[49] Reinoud J Bootsma,et al. Fractional-order information in the visual control of lateral locomotor interception. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] Claire F. Michaels,et al. The optics and actions of catching fly balls , 1992 .
[51] J. Gibson,et al. Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.
[52] M. Wraga. Using eye height in different postures to scale the heights of objects. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[53] Daniel J. Hannon,et al. Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.
[54] Otto Lappi,et al. Visuomotor Control, Eye Movements, and Steering: A Unified Approach for Incorporating Feedback, Feedforward, and Internal Models , 2018, Psychological bulletin.
[55] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Affordance-Based Control of Visually Guided Action , 2007 .
[56] Anne E. Garing,et al. Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] A. V. van den Berg,et al. Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading , 1994, Nature.
[58] David N. Lee,et al. Regulation of gait in long jumping. , 1982 .
[59] J. Perrone,et al. Human Heading Estimation During Visually Simulated Curvilinear Motion , 1997, Vision Research.
[60] Brett R Fajen,et al. Visual Regulation of Gait: Zeroing in on a Solution to the Complex Terrain Problem , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] Seville Chapman. Catching a Baseball , 1968 .
[62] E. Reed. James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception , 1988 .
[63] M. Hayhoe. Vision and Action. , 2017, Annual review of vision science.
[64] Constance S. Royden,et al. From vision to action: experiments and models of steering control during driving. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[65] Frank T. J. M. Zaal,et al. On possibilities for action: The past, present and future of affordance research , 2012 .
[66] J. Bastin,et al. Prospective strategies underlie the control of interceptive actions. , 2006, Human movement science.
[67] Brett R Fajen,et al. Perceiving Possibilities for Action: On the Necessity of Calibration and Perceptual Learning for the Visual Guidance of Action , 2005, Perception.
[68] William H Warren,et al. Follow the leader: visual control of speed in pedestrian following. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[69] L. S. Mark,et al. Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[70] Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al. On the sufficiency of the velocity field for perception of heading , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.
[71] C F Michaels,et al. A note on the natural basis of action categories: the catching distance of mantids. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.
[72] Li Li,et al. No special treatment of independent object motion for heading perception. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[73] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Competitive Dynamics in MSTd: A Mechanism for Robust Heading Perception Based on Optic Flow , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[74] J H Rieger,et al. Processing differential image motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[75] Jing Chen,et al. Influence of visual path information on human heading perception during rotation. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[76] Oliver W. Layton,et al. Possible role for recurrent interactions between expansion and contraction cells in MSTd during self-motion perception in dynamic environments. , 2017, Journal of vision.
[77] D. Proffitt,et al. Eye height scaling of absolute size in immersive and nonimmersive displays. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[78] Betty J. Mohler,et al. Calibration of locomotion resulting from visual motion in a treadmill-based virtual environment , 2007, TAP.
[79] Brett R Fajen,et al. Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies. , 2016, Journal of vision.
[80] K. Cullen. Vestibular processing during natural self-motion: implications for perception and action , 2019, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[81] J. Gibson. Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals , 2009 .
[82] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[83] Kristen L Macuga,et al. Visual control of steering in curve driving. , 2019, Journal of vision.
[84] J. J. Gibson. The theory of affordances , 1977 .
[85] A. W. Blackwell,et al. Perception of circular heading from optical flow. , 1991 .
[86] Brett R Fajen,et al. Reconsidering the role of movement in perceiving action-scaled affordances. , 2011, Human movement science.
[87] J. Saunders. View rotation is used to perceive path curvature from optic flow. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[88] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Guiding locomotion in complex, dynamic environments , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[89] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Intercepting moving targets: a little foresight helps a lot , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[90] Paul R. MacNeilage,et al. Systematic Biases in Human Heading Estimation , 2013, PloS one.
[91] J. Thomson. Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[92] William H. Warren,et al. On-line and model-based approaches to the visual control of action , 2015, Vision Research.
[93] Jessica B. Hamrick,et al. Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[94] W. H. Warren,et al. Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[95] Hector D. Escobar-Alvarez,et al. R‐ADVANCE: Rapid Adaptive Prediction for Vision‐based Autonomous Navigation, Control, and Evasion , 2018, J. Field Robotics.
[96] W. Warren. Collective Motion in Human Crowds , 2018, Current directions in psychological science.
[97] Oliver W. Layton,et al. A motion pooling model of visually guided navigation explains human behavior in the presence of independently moving objects. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[98] M. Kaiser,et al. Optical specification of time-to-passage: Observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993 .
[99] Li Li,et al. Perception of heading during rotation: sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects , 2000, Vision Research.
[100] J. Gibson. The perception of the visual world , 1951 .
[101] M. Turvey,et al. Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. , 2009 .
[102] J. Saunders,et al. Can Observers Judge Future Circular Path Relative to a Target from Retinal Flow? , 2011, Journal of vision.
[103] Kenneth H. Britten,et al. Mechanisms of self-motion perception. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.
[104] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[105] J. Loomis,et al. Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992 .
[106] Jing Chen,et al. Influence of optic-flow information beyond the velocity field on the active control of heading. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[107] Richard M Wilkie,et al. Judgments of path, not heading, guide locomotion. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[108] Stephen Grossberg,et al. A neural model of visually guided steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[109] Yong Gu,et al. Causal Links between Dorsal Medial Superior Temporal Area Neurons and Multisensory Heading Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[110] Simon K Rushton,et al. Does optic flow parsing depend on prior estimation of heading? , 2012, Journal of vision.
[111] M. Graziano,et al. Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[112] K. Adolph. Learning to Move , 2008, Current directions in psychological science.
[113] Mandyam V Srinivasan,et al. Visual control of navigation in insects and its relevance for robotics , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[114] Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al. Perceiving absolute scale in virtual environments: How theory and application have mutually informed the role of body-based perception , 2015 .
[115] Brett R Fajen,et al. Direct perception of action-scaled affordances: the shrinking gap problem. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[116] R. Olberg,et al. Prey size selection and distance estimation in foraging adult dragonflies , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[117] J A Perrone,et al. Model for the computation of self-motion in biological systems. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[118] William H. Warren,et al. Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[119] Dario Floreano,et al. Aerial Locomotion in Cluttered Environments , 2011, ISRR.
[120] James A. Crowell,et al. Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements , 1996, Vision Research.
[121] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Visual and Non-Visual Contributions to the Perception of Object Motion during Self-Motion , 2011, PloS one.
[122] Gilles Montagne,et al. Environmental constraints modify the way an interceptive action is controlled , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[123] H Godthelp,et al. Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.
[124] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Behavioral Dynamics of Visually Guided Locomotion , 2008 .
[125] Brett R. Fajen,et al. From optic flow to laws of control , 2004 .
[126] Jeremy Hsu. Machines on mission possible , 2019 .
[127] Anthony Leonardo,et al. Internal models direct dragonfly interception steering , 2014, Nature.
[128] F. Bremmer,et al. Perception of self-motion from visual flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[129] William H Warren,et al. Catching fly balls in virtual reality: a critical test of the outfielder problem. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[130] James A. Crowell,et al. Estimating heading during eye movements , 1994, Vision Research.
[131] Olaf Blanke,et al. Multisensory Integration in Self Motion Perception , 2016 .
[132] Frank T. J. M. Zaal,et al. When a Fly Ball Is Out of Reach: Catchability Judgments Are Not Based on Optical Acceleration Cancelation , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[133] William H. Warren,et al. Visually Controlled Locomotion: 40 years Later , 1998 .
[134] J M Loomis,et al. Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[135] Li Li,et al. Humans can perceive heading without visual path information. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[136] Melissa S Parade,et al. Humans perceive object motion in world coordinates during obstacle avoidance. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[137] Rongrong Chen,et al. Ability to identify scene-relative object movement is not limited by, or yoked to, ability to perceive heading. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[138] D Regan,et al. How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.
[139] S S Fukusima,et al. Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[140] F. C. Bakker,et al. The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: perception of catchableness of fly balls. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[141] The growing body in action: What infant locomotion tells us about perceptually guided action , 2008 .
[142] W. Warren,et al. Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[143] S. Schaal,et al. Robotics and Neuroscience , 2014, Current Biology.
[144] R. Wurtz,et al. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[145] Heiko Neumann,et al. Modeling heading and path perception from optic flow in the case of independently moving objects , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[146] John P. Wann,et al. Why you should look where you are going , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[147] S. Rushton,et al. Optic Flow Processing for the Assessment of Object Movement during Ego Movement , 2009, Current Biology.
[148] Brett R. Fajen,et al. Controlling speed and direction during interception: an affordance-based approach , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[149] S. Kouider,et al. Increased sensory evidence reverses nonconscious priming during crowding. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[150] G. DeAngelis,et al. A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[151] Li Li,et al. Heading perception from optic flow in the presence of biological motion. , 2019, Journal of vision.
[152] M. Wraga,et al. The role of eye height in perceiving affordances and object dimensions , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[153] Jodie M Plumert,et al. How Do Children Perceive and Act on Dynamic Affordances in Crossing Traffic-Filled Roads? , 2014, Child development perspectives.
[154] Brett R Fajen,et al. Affordance-based versus current-future accounts of choosing whether to pursue or abandon the chase of a moving target , 2020, Journal of vision.
[155] Adhira Sunkara,et al. Joint representation of translational and rotational components of optic flow in parietal cortex , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[156] J H Rieger,et al. Information in optical flows induced by curved paths of observation. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[157] Jack M. Loomis,et al. The importance of perceived relative motion in the control of posture , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[158] W H Warren,et al. Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[159] W. H. Warren,et al. Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[160] Moira B. Flanagan,et al. Movement in the Perception of an Affordance for Wheelchair Locomotion , 2009 .
[161] J. Kelso,et al. Coordination Dynamics: Issues and Trends , 2004, Understanding Complex Systems.
[162] J Godthelp. Precognitive control: open- and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. , 1985, Ergonomics.
[163] A. Chardenon,et al. The visual control of ball interception during human locomotion , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.
[164] J. Rieser,et al. Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.
[165] William H. Warren,et al. Behavioral Dynamics of Heading Alignment in Pedestrian Following , 2014 .
[166] Szonya Durant,et al. The combined effect of eye movements improve head centred local motion information during walking , 2020, PloS one.
[167] Brett R Fajen,et al. The temporal dynamics of heading perception in the presence of moving objects. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[168] G. DeAngelis,et al. Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[169] J Maxwell Donelan,et al. Dynamic Principles of Gait and Their Clinical Implications , 2010, Physical Therapy.
[170] Brett R Fajen,et al. Static and Dynamic Visual Information about the Size and Passability of an Aperture , 2011, Perception.