Visual Control of Locomotion

[1]  C. Michaels,et al.  To Cross or Not to Cross: The Effect of Locomotion on Street-Crossing Behavior , 1996 .

[2]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Perception, Categories, and Possibilities for Action , 2003, Adapt. Behav..

[3]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  A Neural Model of MST and MT Explains Perceived Object Motion during Self-Motion , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Eye Movements and a Rule for Perceiving Direction of Heading , 1999 .

[5]  W. Warren Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action , 1988 .

[6]  W. Warren,et al.  Perception of translational heading from optical flow. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Reinoud J Bootsma The (current) future is here! , 2009, Perception.

[8]  J. Wann,et al.  Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  M. Lenoir,et al.  Intercepting Moving Objects During Self-Motion. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  Jeff Punch,et al.  A visualization of the flow and heat transfer from an oblique impinging jet generated in a square miniature channel , 2016, J. Vis..

[11]  Brett R. Fajen The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking. , 2005 .

[12]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Development of Visually Guided Locomotion , 1998 .

[13]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.

[14]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[15]  William H. Warren,et al.  Chapter 8 – Self-Motion: Visual Perception and Visual Control , 1995 .

[16]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Visual and vestibular cue integration for heading perception in extrastriate visual cortex , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  Li Li,et al.  Perceiving path from optic flow. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[18]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Internal Models in Biological Control , 2019, Annu. Rev. Control. Robotics Auton. Syst..

[19]  J. Loomis,et al.  Model-based control of perception/action , 2004 .

[20]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Multisensory Integration of Visual and Vestibular Signals Improves Heading Discrimination in the Presence of a Moving Object , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  John A Perrone,et al.  Visual-vestibular estimation of the body's curvilinear motion through the world: A computational model. , 2018, Journal of vision.

[23]  Gavan Lintern,et al.  Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997, Complex.

[24]  Michael H Cole,et al.  A systematic review on perceptual-motor calibration to changes in action capabilities. , 2017, Human movement science.

[25]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Causal inference accounts for heading perception in the presence of object motion , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  James A. Balliett,et al.  What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. , 1990 .

[27]  Dees Postma Affordance-based control in running to catch fly balls , 2019 .

[28]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving Heading in the Presence of Moving Objects , 1995, Perception.

[29]  R. Olberg,et al.  Prey pursuit and interception in dragonflies , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[30]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  Analysis of misperceived observer motion during simulated eye rotations , 1994, Vision Research.

[31]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visually Controlled Locomotion: Its Dependence on Optic Flow, Three-Dimensional Space Perception, and Cognition , 1998 .

[32]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  Human heading judgments in the presence of moving objects , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  J. Konczak,et al.  Changing affordances in stair climbing: the perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Constantin A Rothkopf,et al.  The visual control of interceptive steering: How do people steer a car to intercept a moving target? , 2019, Journal of vision.

[35]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003 .

[36]  James A. Crowell,et al.  The perception of heading during eye movements , 1992, Nature.

[37]  M. Turvey Action and perception at the level of synergies. , 2007, Human movement science.

[38]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[39]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[40]  K. J. Craik,et al.  The nature of explanation , 1944 .

[41]  W H Warren,et al.  Visual control of braking: a test of the tau hypothesis. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Frank T J M Zaal,et al.  The Affordance of Catchability in Running to Intercept Fly Balls , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Li Li,et al.  Effects of reference objects and extra-retinal information about pursuit eye movements on curvilinear path perception from retinal flow. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[44]  Matt J. Dunn,et al.  Lateral visual occlusion does not change walking trajectories , 2018, Journal of vision.

[45]  Cynthia F Moss,et al.  Echolocating Bats Use a Nearly Time-Optimal Strategy to Intercept Prey , 2006, PLoS biology.

[46]  Guy Wallis,et al.  An Unexpected Role for Visual Feedback in Vehicle Steering Control , 2002, Current Biology.

[47]  W. H. Warren The dynamics of perception and action. , 2006, Psychological review.

[48]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual Guidance of Intercepting a Moving Target on Foot , 2004, Perception.

[49]  Reinoud J Bootsma,et al.  Fractional-order information in the visual control of lateral locomotor interception. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  Claire F. Michaels,et al.  The optics and actions of catching fly balls , 1992 .

[51]  J. Gibson,et al.  Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.

[52]  M. Wraga Using eye height in different postures to scale the heights of objects. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[54]  Otto Lappi,et al.  Visuomotor Control, Eye Movements, and Steering: A Unified Approach for Incorporating Feedback, Feedforward, and Internal Models , 2018, Psychological bulletin.

[55]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Affordance-Based Control of Visually Guided Action , 2007 .

[56]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading , 1994, Nature.

[58]  David N. Lee,et al.  Regulation of gait in long jumping. , 1982 .

[59]  J. Perrone,et al.  Human Heading Estimation During Visually Simulated Curvilinear Motion , 1997, Vision Research.

[60]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual Regulation of Gait: Zeroing in on a Solution to the Complex Terrain Problem , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  Seville Chapman Catching a Baseball , 1968 .

[62]  E. Reed James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception , 1988 .

[63]  M. Hayhoe Vision and Action. , 2017, Annual review of vision science.

[64]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  From vision to action: experiments and models of steering control during driving. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  Frank T. J. M. Zaal,et al.  On possibilities for action: The past, present and future of affordance research , 2012 .

[66]  J. Bastin,et al.  Prospective strategies underlie the control of interceptive actions. , 2006, Human movement science.

[67]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Perceiving Possibilities for Action: On the Necessity of Calibration and Perceptual Learning for the Visual Guidance of Action , 2005, Perception.

[68]  William H Warren,et al.  Follow the leader: visual control of speed in pedestrian following. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[69]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al.  On the sufficiency of the velocity field for perception of heading , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[71]  C F Michaels,et al.  A note on the natural basis of action categories: the catching distance of mantids. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[72]  Li Li,et al.  No special treatment of independent object motion for heading perception. , 2018, Journal of vision.

[73]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Competitive Dynamics in MSTd: A Mechanism for Robust Heading Perception Based on Optic Flow , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[74]  J H Rieger,et al.  Processing differential image motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[75]  Jing Chen,et al.  Influence of visual path information on human heading perception during rotation. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[76]  Oliver W. Layton,et al.  Possible role for recurrent interactions between expansion and contraction cells in MSTd during self-motion perception in dynamic environments. , 2017, Journal of vision.

[77]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Eye height scaling of absolute size in immersive and nonimmersive displays. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[78]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  Calibration of locomotion resulting from visual motion in a treadmill-based virtual environment , 2007, TAP.

[79]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[80]  K. Cullen Vestibular processing during natural self-motion: implications for perception and action , 2019, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[81]  J. Gibson Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals , 2009 .

[82]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[83]  Kristen L Macuga,et al.  Visual control of steering in curve driving. , 2019, Journal of vision.

[84]  J. J. Gibson The theory of affordances , 1977 .

[85]  A. W. Blackwell,et al.  Perception of circular heading from optical flow. , 1991 .

[86]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Reconsidering the role of movement in perceiving action-scaled affordances. , 2011, Human movement science.

[87]  J. Saunders View rotation is used to perceive path curvature from optic flow. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[88]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Guiding locomotion in complex, dynamic environments , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[89]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Intercepting moving targets: a little foresight helps a lot , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[90]  Paul R. MacNeilage,et al.  Systematic Biases in Human Heading Estimation , 2013, PloS one.

[91]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[92]  William H. Warren,et al.  On-line and model-based approaches to the visual control of action , 2015, Vision Research.

[93]  Jessica B. Hamrick,et al.  Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[94]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[95]  Hector D. Escobar-Alvarez,et al.  R‐ADVANCE: Rapid Adaptive Prediction for Vision‐based Autonomous Navigation, Control, and Evasion , 2018, J. Field Robotics.

[96]  W. Warren Collective Motion in Human Crowds , 2018, Current directions in psychological science.

[97]  Oliver W. Layton,et al.  A motion pooling model of visually guided navigation explains human behavior in the presence of independently moving objects. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[98]  M. Kaiser,et al.  Optical specification of time-to-passage: Observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993 .

[99]  Li Li,et al.  Perception of heading during rotation: sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects , 2000, Vision Research.

[100]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[101]  M. Turvey,et al.  Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. , 2009 .

[102]  J. Saunders,et al.  Can Observers Judge Future Circular Path Relative to a Target from Retinal Flow? , 2011, Journal of vision.

[103]  Kenneth H. Britten,et al.  Mechanisms of self-motion perception. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[104]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[105]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992 .

[106]  Jing Chen,et al.  Influence of optic-flow information beyond the velocity field on the active control of heading. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[107]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  Judgments of path, not heading, guide locomotion. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[108]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A neural model of visually guided steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[109]  Yong Gu,et al.  Causal Links between Dorsal Medial Superior Temporal Area Neurons and Multisensory Heading Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[110]  Simon K Rushton,et al.  Does optic flow parsing depend on prior estimation of heading? , 2012, Journal of vision.

[111]  M. Graziano,et al.  Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[112]  K. Adolph Learning to Move , 2008, Current directions in psychological science.

[113]  Mandyam V Srinivasan,et al.  Visual control of navigation in insects and its relevance for robotics , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[114]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Perceiving absolute scale in virtual environments: How theory and application have mutually informed the role of body-based perception , 2015 .

[115]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Direct perception of action-scaled affordances: the shrinking gap problem. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[116]  R. Olberg,et al.  Prey size selection and distance estimation in foraging adult dragonflies , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[117]  J A Perrone,et al.  Model for the computation of self-motion in biological systems. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[118]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[119]  Dario Floreano,et al.  Aerial Locomotion in Cluttered Environments , 2011, ISRR.

[120]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements , 1996, Vision Research.

[121]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Visual and Non-Visual Contributions to the Perception of Object Motion during Self-Motion , 2011, PloS one.

[122]  Gilles Montagne,et al.  Environmental constraints modify the way an interceptive action is controlled , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[123]  H Godthelp,et al.  Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.

[124]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Behavioral Dynamics of Visually Guided Locomotion , 2008 .

[125]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  From optic flow to laws of control , 2004 .

[126]  Jeremy Hsu Machines on mission possible , 2019 .

[127]  Anthony Leonardo,et al.  Internal models direct dragonfly interception steering , 2014, Nature.

[128]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Perception of self-motion from visual flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[129]  William H Warren,et al.  Catching fly balls in virtual reality: a critical test of the outfielder problem. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[130]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during eye movements , 1994, Vision Research.

[131]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Multisensory Integration in Self Motion Perception , 2016 .

[132]  Frank T. J. M. Zaal,et al.  When a Fly Ball Is Out of Reach: Catchability Judgments Are Not Based on Optical Acceleration Cancelation , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[133]  William H. Warren,et al.  Visually Controlled Locomotion: 40 years Later , 1998 .

[134]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[135]  Li Li,et al.  Humans can perceive heading without visual path information. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[136]  Melissa S Parade,et al.  Humans perceive object motion in world coordinates during obstacle avoidance. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[137]  Rongrong Chen,et al.  Ability to identify scene-relative object movement is not limited by, or yoked to, ability to perceive heading. , 2018, Journal of vision.

[138]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[139]  S S Fukusima,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[140]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: perception of catchableness of fly balls. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[141]  The growing body in action: What infant locomotion tells us about perceptually guided action , 2008 .

[142]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[143]  S. Schaal,et al.  Robotics and Neuroscience , 2014, Current Biology.

[144]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[145]  Heiko Neumann,et al.  Modeling heading and path perception from optic flow in the case of independently moving objects , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[146]  John P. Wann,et al.  Why you should look where you are going , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[147]  S. Rushton,et al.  Optic Flow Processing for the Assessment of Object Movement during Ego Movement , 2009, Current Biology.

[148]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Controlling speed and direction during interception: an affordance-based approach , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[149]  S. Kouider,et al.  Increased sensory evidence reverses nonconscious priming during crowding. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[150]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[151]  Li Li,et al.  Heading perception from optic flow in the presence of biological motion. , 2019, Journal of vision.

[152]  M. Wraga,et al.  The role of eye height in perceiving affordances and object dimensions , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[153]  Jodie M Plumert,et al.  How Do Children Perceive and Act on Dynamic Affordances in Crossing Traffic-Filled Roads? , 2014, Child development perspectives.

[154]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Affordance-based versus current-future accounts of choosing whether to pursue or abandon the chase of a moving target , 2020, Journal of vision.

[155]  Adhira Sunkara,et al.  Joint representation of translational and rotational components of optic flow in parietal cortex , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[156]  J H Rieger,et al.  Information in optical flows induced by curved paths of observation. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[157]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  The importance of perceived relative motion in the control of posture , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[158]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[159]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[160]  Moira B. Flanagan,et al.  Movement in the Perception of an Affordance for Wheelchair Locomotion , 2009 .

[161]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination Dynamics: Issues and Trends , 2004, Understanding Complex Systems.

[162]  J Godthelp Precognitive control: open- and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. , 1985, Ergonomics.

[163]  A. Chardenon,et al.  The visual control of ball interception during human locomotion , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[164]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[165]  William H. Warren,et al.  Behavioral Dynamics of Heading Alignment in Pedestrian Following , 2014 .

[166]  Szonya Durant,et al.  The combined effect of eye movements improve head centred local motion information during walking , 2020, PloS one.

[167]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  The temporal dynamics of heading perception in the presence of moving objects. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[168]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[169]  J Maxwell Donelan,et al.  Dynamic Principles of Gait and Their Clinical Implications , 2010, Physical Therapy.

[170]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Static and Dynamic Visual Information about the Size and Passability of an Aperture , 2011, Perception.