Induced Alpha-band Oscillations Reflect Ratio-dependent Number Discrimination in the Infant Brain

Behavioral studies show that infants are capable of discriminating the number of objects or events in their environment, while also suggesting that number discrimination in infancy may be ratio-dependent. However, due to limitations of the dependent measures used with infant behavioral studies, the evidence for ratio dependence falls short of the vast psychophysical datasets that have established ratio dependence, and thus, adherence to Weber's Law in adults and nonhuman animals. We addressed this issue in two experiments that presented 7-month-old infants with familiar and novel numerosities while electroencephalogram measures of their brain activity were recorded. These data provide convergent evidence that the brains of 7-month-old infants detected numerical novelty. Alpha-band and theta-band oscillations both differed for novel and familiar numerical values. Most importantly, spectral power in the alpha band over midline and right posterior scalp sites was modulated by the ratio between the familiar and novel numerosities. Our findings provide neural evidence that numerical discrimination in infancy is ratio dependent and follows Weber's Law, thus indicating continuity of these cognitive processes over development. Results are also consistent with the idea that networks in the frontal and parietal cortices support ratio-dependent number discrimination in the first year of human life, consistent with what has been reported in neuroimaging studies in adults and older children.

[1]  Paul F Collins,et al.  Neural and behavioral correlates of visual recognition memory in 4- and 8-month-old infants , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  Fei Xu,et al.  Number sense in human infants. , 2005, Developmental science.

[3]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Discrimination of Large and Small Numerosities by Human Infants , 2004 .

[4]  K H Pribram,et al.  Psychophysiological indices of cerebral maturation. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[5]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[6]  C. Nelson,et al.  Recognition of the mother's face by six-month-old infants: a neurobehavioral study. , 1997, Child development.

[7]  E. Basar,et al.  Alpha oscillations in brain functioning: an integrative theory. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[8]  P. Bauer,et al.  Infants' ERP Responses to Novel and Familiar Stimuli Change Over Time: Implications for Novelty Detection and Memory , 2006 .

[9]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[10]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Oscillatory activity in the infant brain reflects object maintenance. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The representation of numerical magnitude , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  J. Cantlon,et al.  Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.

[13]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  O. Bertrand,et al.  Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[16]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Induced alpha band power changes in the human EEG and attention , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  John E Richards,et al.  Attention affects the recognition of briefly presented visual stimuli in infants: an ERP study. , 2003, Developmental science.

[18]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Electrophysiological measures of time processing in infant and adult brains: Weber's law holds , 2008 .

[19]  John E Richards,et al.  Familiarization, attention, and recognition memory in infancy: an event-related potential and cortical source localization study. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[20]  Andrea Berger,et al.  Infant brains detect arithmetic errors , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  T. Striano,et al.  Crossmodal integration of emotional information from face and voice in the infant brain. , 2006, Developmental science.

[22]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Chronometric studies of numerical cognition in five-month-old infants , 2005, Cognition.

[23]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[24]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Electrophysiological Measures of Time Processing in Infant and Adult Brains: Weber's Law Holds , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  M. Elam,et al.  EEG theta rhythm in infants and preschool children , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy , 2004, Cognition.

[27]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[28]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. , 2011, Developmental science.

[29]  Paul C Quinn,et al.  Neural Markers of Categorization in 6-Month-Old Infants , 2006, Psychological science.

[30]  A. Norcia,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to human faces in infants. , 1981, Child Development.

[31]  E. Brannon The independence of language and mathematical reasoning. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  C. Nelson,et al.  Brain activity differentiates face and object processing in 6-month-old infants. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[33]  Justin N Wood,et al.  Infants' enumeration of actions: numerical discrimination and its signature limits. , 2005, Developmental science.

[34]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton–top tamarins , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  T. Striano,et al.  Infants' electric brain responses to emotional prosody , 2005, Neuroreport.

[36]  E. Orekhova,et al.  Theta synchronization during sustained anticipatory attention in infants over the second half of the first year of life. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[37]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants , 2008, PLoS biology.

[38]  R L Maulsby,et al.  An illustration of emotionally evoked theta rhythm in infancy: hedonic hypersynchrony. , 1971, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[39]  E. J. Carter,et al.  Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.

[40]  Elena V Orekhova,et al.  EEG alpha rhythm in infants , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[41]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[42]  P. Starkey,et al.  Perception of numbers by human infants. , 1980, Science.