Plugging Into Word Learning: The Role of Electronic Toys and Digital Media in Language Development

Abstract Digital media and electronic toys are changing the landscape of childhood. How does this change impact language learning? In this chapter, we explore potential alignment between six established principles of language learning and children's engagement with digital media and electronic toys. We argue that electronic toys and digital media are not solely beneficial or solely costly for children's language development. Rather, today's technology has opened up a wide range of new features and possibilities for children's play and media, and each of these affordances must be considered in the context of what research shows us about how children learn language. Certainly, the existing research has shown some ways that these digital media can have negative effects on children's ability to learn, but if used in a thoughtful and responsible way, these sources may be able to be used to have a positive impact on language development.

[1]  Dennis W. Moore,et al.  Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? , 2002 .

[2]  Brian N. Verdine,et al.  Talking Shape: Parental Language with Electronic versus Traditional Shape Sorters. , 2015 .

[3]  Susan Carey,et al.  Acquiring a Single New Word , 1978 .

[4]  Jenny S. Radesky,et al.  Media and Young Minds , 2016, Pediatrics.

[5]  Sarah E. Vaala,et al.  Getting a Read on the App Stores: A Market Scan and Analysis of Children's Literacy Apps. Full Report. , 2015 .

[6]  Shannon M. Pruden,et al.  Children's spatial thinking: does talk about the spatial world matter? , 2011, Developmental science.

[7]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .

[8]  G. Whitehurst,et al.  Accelerating Language Development through Picture Book Reading. , 1988 .

[9]  K. Bloom,et al.  Turn taking affects the quality of infant vocalizations , 1987, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  Adriana G. Bus,et al.  Affordances and limitations of electronic storybooks for young children's emergent literacy , 2015 .

[11]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Supporting toddlers’ transfer of word learning from video , 2014 .

[12]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Active-Constructive-Interactive: A Conceptual Framework for Differentiating Learning Activities , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[13]  C. Ramey,et al.  Home Environment and Cognitive Development in the First 3 Years of Life: A Collaborative Study Involving Six Sites and Three Ethnic Groups in North America , 1989 .

[14]  Adriana G. Bus,et al.  Benefits and Pitfalls of Multimedia and Interactive Features in Technology-Enhanced Storybooks , 2015, Review of educational research.

[15]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Putting Education in “Educational” Apps , 2015, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[17]  Drew P. Cingel,et al.  What kind of adults will our children become? The impact of growing up in a media-saturated world , 2016 .

[18]  G. Bower,et al.  Narrative stories as mediators for serial learning , 1969 .

[19]  K. Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Learning on Hold: Cell Phones Sidetrack Parent-Child Interactions , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[20]  M. Irigoyen,et al.  Exposure and Use of Mobile Media Devices by Young Children , 2015, Pediatrics.

[21]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Do Babies Learn From Baby Media? , 2010, Psychological science.

[22]  David K. Dickinson,et al.  Examining the Acquisition of Vocabulary Knowledge Depth among Preschool Students. , 2016 .

[23]  Lauren A Murphy,et al.  The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. , 2009, Child development.

[24]  K. Clarke-stewart,et al.  Interactions between mothers and their young children: characteristics and consequences. , 1973, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[25]  Paul Fletcher,et al.  Baby's first 10 words. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[26]  A. Biemiller,et al.  An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades , 2006 .

[27]  Elizabeth Lean No More Pencils, No More Books. , 1985 .

[28]  F. Kessel,et al.  The role of syntax in children's comprehension from ages six to twelve. , 1970, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[29]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Early puzzle play: a predictor of preschoolers' spatial transformation skill. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[30]  Ari Brown,et al.  Beyond ‘turn it off’: How to advise families on media use , 2015, AAP News.

[31]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition. , 1997 .

[32]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.

[33]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Taking shape: supporting preschoolers' acquisition of geometric knowledge through guided play. , 2013, Child development.

[34]  Ofra Korat,et al.  How New Technology Influences Parent—child Interaction: The Case of e-book Reading , 2010 .

[35]  C. Snow,et al.  Lexical input as related to children's vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Rebecca J. Panagos Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children , 1998 .

[37]  Alexis R. Lauricella,et al.  Parent-child interactions during traditional and computer storybook reading for children's comprehension: Implications for electronic storybook design , 2014, Int. J. Child Comput. Interact..

[38]  Jenny S. Radesky,et al.  Patterns of Mobile Device Use by Caregivers and Children During Meals in Fast Food Restaurants , 2014, Pediatrics.

[39]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Effective coviewing: Preschoolers' learning from video after a dialogic questioning intervention. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[40]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success , 2015, Psychological science.

[41]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Social Feedback to Infants' Babbling Facilitates Rapid Phonological Learning , 2008, Psychological science.

[42]  Ellen Wartella,et al.  Word learning from baby videos. , 2010, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[43]  Jennifer D. Shapka,et al.  Playing with Technology: Mother-Toddler Interaction Scores Lower during Play with Electronic Toys. , 2012 .

[44]  Monique Sénéchal,et al.  A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: the benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading , 2000 .

[45]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children. , 2008, Child development.

[46]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. , 2014, Child development.

[47]  N. Mahajan,et al.  Once upon a Time: Parent-Child Dialogue and Storybook Reading in the Electronic Era. , 2013 .

[48]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Human simulations of vocabulary learning , 1999, Cognition.

[49]  A. Sosa,et al.  Association of the Type of Toy Used During Play With the Quantity and Quality of Parent-Infant Communication. , 2016, JAMA pediatrics.