The Biology of Signification

Signification is not a term one often meets in biological literature, and few scientists seem to have paid much attention to the question of how there can possibly be such a thing as signification in the world. According to Webster’s Unabridged Dictionary of the English Language (1996), to signify means: “to make known by signs, speech or action” or “to be a sign of.” That signification in this sense is constitutive for human life is hard to deny, since the very act of denying it would itself be a linguistic and thus significative act. Being linguistic animals, we are incurably suspended in a world of signification. But language is not the only or even the primary key to the world of signification. Small children do not speak, but it would be absurd to claim that they are not yet conscious about things around them. Most of the time, in fact, our understanding of what goes on is based on the myriad of nonlinguistic signs entering our perceptive fields. Even when our consciousness is not aware of it, our body and brain continue to interpret the world around us, alerting our consciousness only when necessary. The science devoted to the exploration of this world of signification is called or the theory of signs or semiotics (from the Greek for “sign,” sēmeion). Due to tradition and to the influential work of the Swiss linguist Ferdinand de Saussure, who was exclusively concerned with that peculiar sign system which is language [1], semiotics generally has been considered a part of the humanities, but there seems to be no good reason to believe that only human beings are admitted into the world of signification. The existence of signification therefore challenges biological theory to find out what are the roots for this phenomenon in pre-human nature. This challenge has been met often enough by a reductive strategy, in which semiotic processes are identified as signals unambiguously releas-

[1]  H. Jane Brockmann,et al.  The selfish gene (2nd edn) , 1990 .

[2]  R. Ulanowicz,et al.  Evolution in thermodynamic perspective: An ecological approach , 1989 .

[3]  S. Lang,et al.  Collected Papers IV , 2000 .

[4]  A. Clark Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again , 1996 .

[5]  T. Sebeok COMMUNICATION IN ANIMALS AND IN MEN; THREE REVIEWS , 1968 .

[6]  Jesper Hoffmeyer,et al.  Surfaces inside surfaces. On the origin of agency and life , 1998, Cybern. Hum. Knowing.

[7]  John Deely,et al.  Basics of semiotics , 1990 .

[8]  John R. Searle,et al.  The Rediscovery of the Mind , 1995, Artif. Intell..

[9]  R. Rosen Life Itself: A Comprehensive Inquiry Into the Nature, Origin, and Fabrication of Life , 1991 .

[10]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Contributions of vision and proprioception to accuracy in limb movements , 1994 .

[11]  Karl R. Popper,et al.  A World of Propensities , 1993, Popper's Views on Natural and Social Science.

[12]  A. Rayner Degrees of Freedom: Living in Dynamic Boundaries , 1997 .

[13]  L. D. Tsitsipis,et al.  Universe of the Mind: A Semiotic Theory of Culture , 1993 .

[14]  W. N. Kellogg,et al.  Communication among social bees@@@Porpoises and Sonar@@@Man and Dolphin , 1963 .

[15]  F. D. Saussure Cours de linguistique générale , 1924 .

[16]  C. Duve Blueprint for a Cell: The Nature and Origin of Life , 1991 .

[17]  J. Sapp Evolution by association : a history of symbiosis , 1994 .

[18]  Order out of indeterminacy , 1999 .

[19]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[20]  Thomas A. Sebeok,et al.  The Sign and Its Masters , 1980 .

[21]  Bruce H. Weber,et al.  Darwinism Evolving: Systems Dynamics and the Genealogy of Natural Selection , 1994 .

[22]  O. Sacks A leg to stand on , 1984 .

[23]  Thomas A. Sebeok,et al.  Contributions to the doctrine of signs , 1976 .

[24]  R. Ulanowicz Ecology, the ascendent perspective , 1997 .

[25]  The Sign and Its Masters , 1980 .

[26]  L. Braga A new causality for the understanding of the living , 1999 .

[27]  Brian C. Goodwin,et al.  How the Leopard Changed Its Spots , 1995 .