Direct feedback and social conformity promote behavioral change via mechanisms indexed by centroparietal positivity: Electrophysiological evidence from a role-swapping ultimatum game.

Our behavior is shaped by multiple factors, including direct feedback (seeing the outcomes of our past actions) and social observation (in part, via a drive to conform to other peoples' behaviors). However, it remains unclear how these two processes are linked in the context of behavioral change. This is important to investigate, as behavioral change is associated with distinct neural correlates that reflect specific aspects of processing, such as information integration and rule updating. To clarify whether these processes characterize both direct learning and conformity, we elicited the two within the same task, using a role-swapping version of the Ultimatum Game-a fairness paradigm where subjects decide how to share a pot of money with other players-while electroencephalography (EEG) data were recorded. Behavioral results showed that subjects decided how to divide the pot based on both direct feedback (seeing whether their past proposals were accepted or rejected) and social observation (copying the splits that others just proposed). Converging EEG evidence revealed that increased centroparietal positivity (P2, P3b, and late positivity) indexed behavioral changes motivated by direct feedback and those motivated by drives to conform. However, exploratory analyses also suggest that these two motivating factors may also be dissociable, and that frontal midline theta oscillations may predict behavioral changes linked to direct feedback but not conformity. Overall, this study provides novel electrophysiological evidence regarding the different forms of behavioral change. These findings are also relevant for understanding the mechanisms of social information processing that underlie successful cooperation.

[1]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Prosocial Influence and Opportunistic Conformity in Adolescents and Young Adults , 2020, Psychological science.

[2]  Jutta Peterburs,et al.  Asymmetric coupling of action and outcome valence in active and observational feedback learning , 2020, Psychological research.

[3]  Thomas D. Sambrook,et al.  Evidence for parietal reward prediction errors using great grand average meta-analysis. , 2020, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[4]  G. Hajcak,et al.  Significance?... Significance! Empirical, methodological, and theoretical connections between the late positive potential and P300 as neural responses to stimulus significance: An integrative review. , 2020, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Ewelina Knapska,et al.  The neural and computational systems of social learning , 2020, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[6]  Shinsuke Suzuki,et al.  Testing the reinforcement learning hypothesis of social conformity , 2020, bioRxiv.

[7]  H. Cheung,et al.  Feedback-related Negativity (FRN) and Theta Oscillations: Different Feedback Signals for Non-Conform and Conform Decisions , 2020, Biological Psychology.

[8]  R. Ebstein,et al.  To run with the herd or not: Electrophysiological dynamics are associated with preference change in crowdfunding , 2019, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  P. Montoya,et al.  Time Course of the Neural Activity Related to Behavioral Decision-Making as Revealed by Event-Related Potentials , 2019, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[10]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Trait anger and the reward positivity , 2019, Personality and Individual Differences.

[11]  L. Meteyard,et al.  Best practice guidance for linear mixed-effects models in psychological science , 2019, Journal of Memory and Language.

[12]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Herding Brains: A Core Neural Mechanism for Social Alignment , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  Joshua M. Carlson,et al.  No Money, No Problem: Enhanced Reward Positivity in the Absence of Monetary Reward , 2019, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Luke J. Chang,et al.  Expectations in the Ultimatum Game: Distinct Effects of Mean and Variance of Expected Offers , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[15]  E. Valadez,et al.  An electrophysiological index of outcome evaluation that may influence subsequent cooperation and aggression strategies , 2018, Social neuroscience.

[16]  Takako Fujioka,et al.  Performance monitoring of self and other in a turn-taking piano duet: A dual-EEG study , 2018, Social neuroscience.

[17]  Oriel FeldmanHall,et al.  Learning Moral Values: Another’s Desire to Punish Enhances One’s Own Punitive Behavior , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  Megan A. Boudewyn,et al.  How many trials does it take to get a significant ERP effect? It depends. , 2018, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[20]  Boris C. Bernhardt,et al.  Structural plasticity of the social brain: Differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training , 2017, Science Advances.

[21]  Spencer D. Kelly,et al.  Body posture and gender impact neural processing of power-related words , 2017, The Journal of social psychology.

[22]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates reflect the integration of model-based and model-free decision information , 2017, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  Alexandre Gramfort,et al.  Autoreject: Automated artifact rejection for MEG and EEG data , 2016, NeuroImage.

[24]  Haiyan Wu,et al.  Neural signatures of social conformity: A coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[25]  Steven G. Luke,et al.  Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R , 2016, Behavior Research Methods.

[26]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reward positivity: Reward prediction error or salience prediction error? , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[27]  D. Foti,et al.  The temporal dynamics of reversal learning: P3 amplitude predicts valence-specific behavioral adjustment , 2016, Physiology & Behavior.

[28]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  The involvement of model-based but not model-free learning signals during observational reward learning in the absence of choice. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Dennis J L G Schutter,et al.  Anger is associated with reward-related electrocortical activity: Evidence from the reward positivity. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[30]  G. Juckel,et al.  Performance monitoring and empathy during active and observational learning in patients with major depression , 2015, Biological Psychology.

[31]  Jamil Zaki,et al.  Common and distinct neural correlates of personal and vicarious reward: A quantitative meta-analysis , 2015, NeuroImage.

[32]  J. Cavanagh,et al.  Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[33]  Christian Bellebaum,et al.  From Feedback- to Response-based Performance Monitoring in Active and Observational Learning , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Frank,et al.  Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Michael Siniatchkin,et al.  Visual event-related potentials to biological motion stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[36]  Ernest Mas-Herrero,et al.  Frontal Theta Oscillatory Activity Is a Common Mechanism for the Computation of Unexpected Outcomes and Learning Rate , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Martin Luessi,et al.  MNE software for processing MEG and EEG data , 2014, NeuroImage.

[38]  Martin Luessi,et al.  MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python , 2013, Front. Neuroinform..

[39]  Yong Zheng,et al.  Neural mechanisms underlying social conformity in an ultimatum game , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  C. Bellebaum,et al.  Empathy and feedback processing in active and observational learning , 2013, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Rapid Brain Responses Independently Predict Gain Maximization and Loss Minimization during Economic Decision Making , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[43]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  How does noise affect amplitude and latency measurement of event-related potentials (ERPs)? A methodological critique and simulation study. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[44]  T. Lohrenz,et al.  Computational Substrates of Norms and Their Violations during Social Exchange , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Corinna N. Scheel,et al.  You don’t like me, do you? Enhanced ERP responses to averted eye gaze in social anxiety , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[46]  Norbert Kathmann,et al.  Neural correlates of feedback processing in decision-making under risk , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[47]  Leonie Koban,et al.  Effects of social context and predictive relevance on action outcome monitoring , 2012, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[48]  I. Daum,et al.  The neural coding of expected and unexpected monetary performance outcomes: Dissociations between active and observational learning , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[49]  Luke J. Chang,et al.  Great expectations: neural computations underlying the use of social norms in decision-making. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[50]  R. Dolan,et al.  Optimistic biases in observational learning of value , 2011, Cognition.

[51]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Feedback-related Negativity Codes Prediction Error but Not Behavioral Adjustment during Probabilistic Reversal Learning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  Christian Bellebaum,et al.  It was not MY fault: event-related brain potentials in active and observational learning from feedback. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  W. Schultz,et al.  Neural mechanisms of observational learning , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Y. Niv Reinforcement learning in the brain , 2009 .

[55]  G. Fernández,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity , 2009, Neuron.

[56]  H. Schielzeth,et al.  Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models , 2008, Behavioral ecology : official journal of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology.

[57]  Mark W Woolrich,et al.  Associative learning of social value , 2008, Nature.

[58]  N. Sadato,et al.  Processing of Social and Monetary Rewards in the Human Striatum , 2008, Neuron.

[59]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[60]  Cristina Bicchieri,et al.  Do the Right Thing: But Only If Others Do So , 2007 .

[61]  D. Linden The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[62]  H. Oosterbeek,et al.  Cultural Differences in Ultimatum Game Experiments: Evidence from a Meta-Analysis , 2001 .

[63]  Madan M. Pillutla,et al.  Unfairness, Anger, and Spite: Emotional Rejections of Ultimatum Offers , 1996 .

[64]  A. Tversky,et al.  Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty , 1992 .

[65]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[66]  J. Varni,et al.  An analysis of observational learning in autistic and normal children , 1979, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[67]  Heather C. Lench,et al.  Exploring the Toolkit of Emotion: What Do Sadness and Anger Do for Us? , 2016 .

[68]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Committee report: publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[69]  Marco Faillo,et al.  Conformity, reciprocity and the sense of justice. How social contract-based preferences and beliefs explain norm compliance: the experimental evidence , 2010 .

[70]  Hawaii,et al.  Supporting Online Material Materials and Methods Figs. S1 to S6 Tables S1 and S2 Database S1 Antisocial Punishment across Societies , 2022 .