Influence of dynamic tilts on the perception of earth-vertical

Abstract.The aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that optimal activation of both the semicircular canals and the otoliths provides reliable vestibular cues about self-orientation in space. For this, we measured the ability of subjects to estimate the subjective vertical immediately, 20 s and 90 s after a rapid tilt (180°/s2) from upright into different roll positions between 90° left and right side down. Subjects had to estimate the earth-vertical and earth-horizontal direction in the dark by (a) setting a luminous line, (b) performing saccades, and (c) verbally declaring body position relative to gravity. The mean error curves from the three paradigms showed consistent E (Müller)- and A (Aubert)-effects, which did not significantly change over time. Horizontal and vertical saccade tasks exhibited different response characteristics, as previously reported by others, which likely reflect different computation mechanisms. The verbal estimation paradigm yielded complementary results to those of the luminous line paradigm and vertical saccade task. The E-effect of the luminous line and the vertical saccade paradigm might be explained by a bias towards earth-vertical due to interactions of vestibular and neck afferent signals. The invariably small A-effect of the luminous line and the vertical saccade paradigm probably reflects somatosensory signals that had relatively weak influence in our experiments. We conclude that phasic activation of the vestibular system reduces the influence of non-vestibular cues observed in low tilt velocity or static experiments. Although this activation generates an E-effect, the total error in the range of ±90° is reduced.

[1]  Werner Haustein,et al.  Considerations on Listing's Law and the primary position by means of a matrix description of eye position control , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  H Mittelstaedt,et al.  New diagnostic tests for the function of utricles, saccules and somatic graviceptors. , 1995, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[3]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. I. Resting discharge and response to constant angular accelerations. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  I. Curthoys,et al.  Visually perceived vertical and visually perceived horizontal are not orthogonal , 1998, Vision Research.

[5]  S G Diamond,et al.  A predictive test for space motion sickness. , 1993, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[6]  V. J. Wilson,et al.  Mammalian Vestibular Physiology , 1979, Springer US.

[7]  I. Curthoys,et al.  The Effect of Ocular Torsional Position on Perception of the Roll-tilt of Visual Stimuli , 1997, Vision Research.

[8]  D. Straumann,et al.  Transient torsion during and after saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. 3. Variations among units in their discharge properties. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  B J Hess,et al.  Computation of Inertial Motion: Neural Strategies to Resolve Ambiguous Otolith Information , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  G T Gdowski,et al.  Firing behavior of vestibular neurons during active and passive head movements: vestibulo-spinal and other non-eye-movement related neurons. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  A. Bronstein,et al.  A case of thalamic syndrome: somatosensory influences on visual orientation , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[13]  Fred Mast,et al.  Perceived body position and the visual horizontal , 1996, Brain Research Bulletin.

[14]  A. D. Van Beuzekom,et al.  Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates. , 2000 .

[15]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Horizontal eye position-related activity in neck muscles of the alert cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  I. Israël,et al.  Vestibular perception of passive whole-body rotation about horizontal and vertical axes in humans: goal-directed vestibulo-ocular reflex and vestibular memory-contingent saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  A. Savitzky,et al.  Smoothing and Differentiation of Data by Simplified Least Squares Procedures. , 1964 .

[18]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III. Response dynamics. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  G M Gauthier,et al.  Perception of passive whole-body rotations in the absence of neck and body proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  B. Day,et al.  Control of the head in response to tilt of the body in normal and labyrinthine‐defective human subjects. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  N. Dieringer,et al.  The role of compensatory eye and head movements in the rat for image stabilization and gaze orientation , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  F E Guedry,et al.  The effect of semicircular cana stimulation during tilting on the subsequent perception of the visual vertical. , 1970, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[23]  M. A. Gresty,et al.  Assessment of the perception of verticality and horizontality with self-paced saccades , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  A M Bronstein,et al.  The perception of head and neck angular displacement in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects. A quantitative study using a 'remembered saccade' technique. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  H. Mittelstaedt A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical , 1983, Naturwissenschaften.

[27]  Millard F. Reschke,et al.  Spatial coding of eye movements relative to perceived earth and head orientations during static roll tilt , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Jefferson E. Roy,et al.  Selective Processing of Vestibular Reafference during Self-Generated Head Motion , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Stefano Corna,et al.  The functional effectiveness of neck muscle reflexes for head-righting in response to sudden fall , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Michael Fetter,et al.  The interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation to subjective “straight ahead” orientation in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  R J Peterka,et al.  Torsional vestibulo-ocular reflex measurements for identifying otolith asymmetries possibly related to space motion sickness susceptibility. , 1994, Acta astronautica.

[32]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  The role of the otoliths in the perception of the orientation of self and world to the vertical , 1991 .

[33]  F. A. Seiler,et al.  Numerical Recipes in C: The Art of Scientific Computing , 1989 .

[34]  Ian S. Curthoys,et al.  The role of ocular torsion in visual measures of vestibular function , 1996, Brain Research Bulletin.

[35]  G. Paige,et al.  Eye movement responses to linear head motion in the squirrel monkey. I. Basic characteristics. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  F. Guedry Psychophysics of Vestibular Sensation , 1974 .

[37]  T Mergner,et al.  Vestibular memory‐contingent saccades involve somatosensory input from the body support , 1998, Neuroreport.

[38]  R. Mayne,et al.  A Systems Concept of the Vestibular Organs , 1974 .

[39]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  The information processing structure of the subjective vertical. A cybernetic bridge between its psychophysics and its neurobiology , 1988 .