Materials count: Linear-spatial materials improve young children’s addition strategies and accuracy, irregular arrays don’t

Children who use advanced arithmetic strategies, such as count-on and decomposition, are more accurate when solving arithmetic problems and are more likely to later have higher levels of math achievement. The present study tested the hypothesis that instruction using linear-spatial representations would activate children’s knowledge necessary for use of mental addition strategies and, thus, lead to greater accuracy on addition problems, than instruction using irregular representations of magnitude. As predicted, low-income kindergartners (n = 29) randomly assigned to practice sums up to 10 using materials that instantiated the linear-spatial features of a mental number line (i.e., discrete squares arranged in rows) demonstrated substantially more improvement in solving unpracticed addition problems than children who practiced with irregular materials (i.e., pictures of stars arranged in random arrays). This was particularly true for children with better initial numerical knowledge, which provided support for the idea that existing knowledge was activated. The use of count-on more than doubled from pretest to posttest among children in the linear-spatial condition and this mediated the difference in improvement between conditions. The importance of aligning instructional materials to relevant mental representations–consistent with the Cognitive Alignment Framework for instructional design—is discussed.

[1]  K. Mccartney,et al.  Effect size, practical importance, and social policy for children. , 2000, Child development.

[2]  Jennifer Dobbs,et al.  Accelerating Math Development in Head Start Classrooms , 2002 .

[3]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  A Two-Minute Paper-and-Pencil Test of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing Explains Variability in Primary School Children's Arithmetic Competence , 2013, PloS one.

[4]  Susan R. Goldman,et al.  Extended Practice of Basic Addition Facts: Strategy Changes in Learning-Disabled Students , 1988 .

[5]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  EFFICIENCY AND ADAPTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE SCHOOL- TAUGHT STRATEGIES IN THE DOMAIN OF SIMPLE ADDITION , 2004 .

[7]  Eileen Wood,et al.  Encouraging Mindful Use of Prior Knowledge: Attempting to Construct Explanatory Answers Facilitates Learning , 1992 .

[8]  D. Geary,et al.  Counting Knowledge and Skill in Cognitive Addition: a Comparison of Normal and Mathematically Disabled Children , 2022 .

[9]  Koleen McCrink,et al.  Development of Spatial-Numerical Associations , 2014, Current directions in psychological science.

[10]  Mary Jo Kane,et al.  Young children's mental models determine analogical transfer across problems with a common goal structure * , 1986 .

[11]  R. Siegler The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children's addition. , 1987 .

[12]  Bethany Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Easy as ABCABC: Abstract Language Facilitates Performance on a Concrete Patterning Task. , 2015, Child development.

[13]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.

[14]  Dc Washington National Governors Association and Chief Council of State School Officers. , 2010 .

[15]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.

[16]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Calculation Abilities in Young Children with Different Patterns of Cognitive Functioning , 1995, Journal of learning disabilities.

[17]  H. Tavakoli The Relationship Between Accuracy of Numerical Magnitude Comparisons and Children’s Arithmetic Ability: A Study in Iranian Primary School Children , 2016, Europe's journal of psychology.

[18]  D. Gentner,et al.  Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. , 1997 .

[19]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Children's understanding of counting , 1990, Cognition.

[20]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .

[21]  J. Moss,et al.  Enhancing Children's Spatial and Numerical Skills through a Dynamic Spatial Approach to Early Geometry Instruction: Effects of a 32-Week Intervention , 2017 .

[22]  Barbara J. Guzzetti,et al.  Promoting conceptual change in science: A comparative meta-analysis of instructional interventions from reading education and science education , 1993 .

[23]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Development of calculation abilities in middle- and low-income children after formal instruction in school☆ , 1994 .

[24]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Symbolic Number Skills Predict Growth in Nonsymbolic Number Skills in Kindergarteners , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[25]  Z. Chen,et al.  Children's analogical problem solving: the effects of superficial, structural, and procedural similarity. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[26]  Emily R. Fyfe,et al.  Benefits of practicing 4 = 2 + 2: nontraditional problem formats facilitate children's understanding of mathematical equivalence. , 2011, Child development.

[27]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic , 2008, Cognition.

[28]  J. Moss,et al.  Effects of mental rotation training on children’s spatial and mathematics performance: A randomized controlled study , 2015, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[29]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Numerical estimation in preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[30]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Playing linear number board games—but not circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. , 2009 .

[31]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Arithmetic Accuracy in Children From High- and Low-Income Schools , 2016 .

[32]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Reducing the gap in numerical knowledge between low- and middle-income preschoolers , 2011 .

[33]  Hannah M. Nash,et al.  Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? , 2016, Developmental science.

[34]  Spreadsheets, Pedagogic Strategies and the Evolution of Meaning for Variable , 2005 .

[35]  G. Duncan,et al.  School readiness and later achievement. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Number line estimation and mental addition: examining the potential roles of language and education. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[37]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Numerical Magnitude Representations and Individual Differences in Children's Arithmetic Strategy Use , 2012 .

[38]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Early math matters: kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[39]  R. Newman,et al.  Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. , 1986, Child development.

[40]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[41]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[42]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[43]  W. Schneider The Development of Metacognitive Knowledge in Children and Adolescents: Major Trends and Implications for Education , 2008 .

[44]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  What basic number processing measures in kindergarten explain unique variability in first-grade arithmetic proficiency? , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[45]  J. LeFevre,et al.  Learning to Count: Structured Practice With Spatial Cues Supports the Development of Counting Sequence Knowledge in 3-Year-Old English-Speaking Children , 2017 .

[46]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[47]  Aki Murata Paths to Learning Ten-Structured Understandings of Teen Sums: Addition Solution Methods of Japanese Grade 1 Students , 2004 .

[48]  Vimla L. Patel,et al.  Explanatory Models in the Processing of Science Text: The Role of Prior Knowledge Activation Through Small-Group Discussion , 1989 .

[49]  S. Vosniadou,et al.  Designing learning environments to promote conceptual change in science , 2001 .

[50]  D. Geary,et al.  Cognitive addition: strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in young and elderly adults. , 1991, Psychology and aging.

[51]  Eric A. Jenkins,et al.  How Children Discover New Strategies , 1989 .

[52]  D. Geary,et al.  Development of arithmetical competencies in Chinese and American children: influence of age, language, and schooling. , 1996, Child development.

[53]  J. LeFevre,et al.  Pathways to mathematics: longitudinal predictors of performance. , 2010, Child development.

[54]  James M. Moser,et al.  The Acquisition of Addition and Subtraction Concepts in Grades One through Three. , 1984 .

[55]  D. Geary,et al.  A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[56]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Number sense growth in kindergarten: a longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. , 2006, Child development.

[57]  Hilary C Barth,et al.  The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.

[58]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[59]  Susan C. Levine,et al.  The Development of Proportional Reasoning: Effect of Continuous Versus Discrete Quantities , 2007 .

[60]  D. Geary Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: a 5-year longitudinal study. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[61]  D. Gentner,et al.  Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: young children's use of comparison in category learning. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[62]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Adolescents’ Functional Numeracy Is Predicted by Their School Entry Number System Knowledge , 2013, PloS one.

[63]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. , 2008, Developmental science.

[64]  E. Ceulemans,et al.  Profiles of children's arithmetic fact development: a model-based clustering approach. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[65]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Learning to spell: variability, choice, and change in children's strategy use. , 1999, Child development.

[66]  Andrew F. Hayes,et al.  Mediation and the Estimation of Indirect Effects in Political Communication Research , 2015 .

[67]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[68]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Learning from number board games: you learn what you encode. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[69]  A. Kappers,et al.  Tactile perception of thermal diffusivity , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[70]  R. Siegler Emerging Minds: The Process of Change in Children's Thinking , 1996 .

[71]  Koen Lamberts,et al.  The encoding–retrieval relationship: retrieval as mental simulation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[72]  Jeff Shrager,et al.  SCADS: A Model of Children's Strategy Choices and Strategy Discoveries , 1998 .