The generation of secondary saccades without postsaccadic visual feedback.

Primary saccades are often followed by small secondary saccades, which are generally thought to reduce the distance between the saccade endpoint and target location. Accumulated evidence demonstrates that secondary saccades are subject to various influences, among which retinal feedback during postsaccadic fixation constitutes only one important signal. Recently, we reported that target eccentricity and an orientation bias influence the generation of secondary saccades. In the present study, we examine secondary saccades in the absence of postsaccadic visual feedback. Although extraretinal signals (e.g., efference copy) have received widespread attention in eye-movement studies, it is still unclear whether an extraretinal error signal contributes to the programming of secondary saccades. We have observed that secondary saccade latency and amplitude depend on primary saccade error despite the absence of postsaccadic visual feedback. Strong evidence for an extraretinal error signal influencing secondary saccade programming is given by the observation that secondary saccades are more likely to be oriented in a direction opposite to the primary saccade as primary saccade error shifts from target undershoot to overshoot. We further show how the functional relationship between primary saccade landing position and secondary saccade characteristics varies as a function of target eccentricity. We propose that initial target eccentricity and an extraretinal error signal codetermine the postsaccadic activity distribution in the saccadic motor map when no visual feedback is available.

[1]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[2]  W. Shebilske Extraretinal information in corrective saccades and inflow vs outflow theories of visual direction constancy , 1976, Vision Research.

[3]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Secondary (micro-)saccades: The influence of primary saccade end point and target eccentricity on the process of postsaccadic fixation , 2011, Vision Research.

[4]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades Keep the Eyes' Balance During Fixation , 2004, Psychological science.

[5]  R. Klein,et al.  Aftereffects of Saccades Explored in a Dynamic Neural Field Model of the Superior Colliculus , 2011 .

[6]  Julie D. Golomb,et al.  The Native Coordinate System of Spatial Attention Is Retinotopic , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Pierre Morel,et al.  Optimal and Suboptimal Use of Postsaccadic Vision in Sequences of Saccades , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.

[9]  D. Henson Corrective saccades: Effects of altering visual feedback , 1978, Vision Research.

[10]  L Stark,et al.  Closely spaced saccades. , 1975, Investigative ophthalmology.

[11]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Extraretinal signal metrics in multiple-saccade sequences. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[12]  M. Rolfs Microsaccades: Small steps on a long way , 2009, Vision Research.

[13]  Z. Kapoula,et al.  Evidence for a range effect in the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.

[14]  Ziad M. Hafed Mechanisms for generating and compensating for the smallest possible saccades , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[15]  R. Wurtz Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability , 2008, Vision Research.

[16]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation , 2005, Neuron.

[17]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[18]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[20]  M. Rolfs,et al.  Visual suppression in the superior colliculus around the time of microsaccades. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  Pierre Baraduc,et al.  Saccadic adaptation without retinal postsaccadic error , 2007, Neuroreport.

[23]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Retinal eccentricity and the latency of eye saccades , 1994, Vision Research.

[24]  W. Peddie,et al.  Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1924, Nature.

[25]  Reinhold Kliegl,et al.  Experimental Effects and Individual Differences in Linear Mixed Models: Estimating the Relationship between Spatial, Object, and Attraction Effects in Visual Attention , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[26]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention , 2005, Vision Research.

[27]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[28]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[29]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Amplitudes and directions of individual saccades can be adjusted by corollary discharge. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[30]  R B Daroff,et al.  Corrective movements following refixation saccades: type and control system analysis. , 1972, Vision research.

[31]  D. Robinson,et al.  Saccadic undershoot is not inevitable: Saccades can be accurate , 1986, Vision Research.

[32]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkey during a Covert Visual Attention Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[34]  James J. Clark,et al.  Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.

[35]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Corrective saccades: dependence on retinal reafferent signals , 1975, Vision Research.

[36]  W. Wolf,et al.  Corrective saccades: Effect of shifting the saccade goal , 1982, Vision Research.

[37]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[38]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Post-saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non-foveal locations. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[39]  Q. Zaidi,et al.  The utility of shape attributes in deciphering movements of non-rigid objects. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[40]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[41]  Erik Blaser,et al.  The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets , 1995, Vision Research.

[42]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Differences in accuracy of human saccades between stationary and jumping targets , 1989, Vision Research.

[43]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Microsaccadic Suppression of Visual Bursts in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.