Perceiving action-relevant properties of tools through dynamic touch: effects of mass distribution, exploration style, and intention.

At issue in the present series of experiments was the ability to prospectively perceive the action-relevant properties of hand-held tools by means of dynamic touch. In Experiment 1, participants judged object move-ability. In Experiment 2, participants judged how difficult an object would be to hold if held horizontally, and in Experiments 3 and 4, participants rated how fast objects could be rotated. In each experiment, the first and second moments of mass distribution of the objects were systematically varied. Manipulations of wielding speed and orientation during restricted exploration revealed perception to be constrained by (a) the moments of mass distribution of the hand-tool system, (b) the qualities of exploratory wielding movements, and (c) the intention to perceive each specific property. The results are considered in the context of the ecological theory of dynamic touch. Implications for accounts of the informational basis of dynamic touch and for the development of a theory of haptically perceiving the affordance properties of tools are discussed.

[1]  Robert E. Shaw,et al.  Ecological mechanics: A physical geometry for intentional constraints , 1988 .

[2]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Effortful touch with minimal movement , 1992 .

[3]  Morton A. Heller,et al.  Touch, representation, and blindness , 2000 .

[4]  Susan J. Lederman,et al.  Extracting object properties through haptic exploration. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[5]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[6]  Kevin Shockley,et al.  Perceiving Affordances of Hockey Sticks by Dynamic Touch , 2006 .

[7]  M. Turvey,et al.  Explaining Metamers: Right Degrees of Freedom, Not Subjectivism , 2001, Consciousness and Cognition.

[8]  J. Gibson Observations on active touch. , 1962, Psychological review.

[9]  M T Turvey,et al.  Perceiving the width and height of a hand-held object by dynamic touch. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  M T Turvey,et al.  Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  M. Turvey Action and perception at the level of synergies. , 2007, Human movement science.

[12]  Idsart Kingma,et al.  Perception of limb orientation in the vertical plane depends on center of mass rather than inertial eigenvectors , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Claudia Carello,et al.  Perceiving Affordances by Dynamic Touch: Hints From the Control of Movement , 2004 .

[14]  Christopher C. Pagano,et al.  SPINORS AND SELECTIVE DYNAMIC TOUCH , 1996 .

[15]  M. Turvey,et al.  Affordance, proper function, and the physical basis of perceived heaviness , 1999, Cognition.

[16]  Kevin Shockley,et al.  Metamers in the haptic perception of heaviness and moveableness , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Affordances and Inertial Constraints on Tool Use , 2001 .

[18]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .

[19]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Gravitational and Muscular Variables in Perceiving Rod Extent by Wielding , 1989 .

[20]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[22]  J G Kreifeldt,et al.  Moment of inertia: psychophysical study of an overlooked sensation. , 1979, Science.

[23]  G. Bingham,et al.  Hefting for a maximum distance throw: a smart perceptual mechanism. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Steven J. Harrison,et al.  Comparison of Dynamic (Effortful) Touch by Hand and Foot , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  M T Turvey,et al.  Can shape be perceived by dynamic touch? , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Perceiving the Lengths of Rods That are Held But Not Wielded , 1990 .

[27]  M. Turvey,et al.  Weight perception and the haptic size-weight illusion are functions of the inertia tensor. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  M. Turvey,et al.  Somatosensory attunement to the rigid body laws , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  S. R. Ganeshan,et al.  Effortful touch with minimum movement: revisited. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  M T Turvey,et al.  Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  Jeffrey B Wagman,et al.  Chosen striking location and the user-tool-environment system. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[32]  M. Turvey,et al.  Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense” , 1994, Neuroscience.

[33]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Links Between Active Perception and the Control of Action , 1990 .

[34]  Robert E. Shaw,et al.  Dimensionless invariants for intentional systems: Measuring the fit of vehicular activities to environmental layout. , 1995 .

[35]  M. Turvey,et al.  Ecological laws of perceiving and acting: In reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn (1981) , 1981, Cognition.

[36]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Perceiving extents of rods by wielding: haptic diagonalization and decomposition of the inertia tensor. , 1989 .

[37]  Steven J. Harrison,et al.  Using Vision and Dynamic Touch to Perceive the Affordances of Tools , 2007, Perception.

[38]  A. Noē Direct Perception , 2022 .

[39]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Mechanical invariants are implicated in dynamic touch as a function of their salience in the stimulus flow. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  I. Kingma,et al.  The Inertia Tensor Versus Static Moment and Mass in Perceiving Length and Heaviness of Hand-Wielded Rods , 2002 .

[41]  Robert E. Shaw,et al.  Modelling Systems with Intentional Dynamics: A Lesson from Quantum Mechanics , 2018, Origins.

[42]  Michael A Riley,et al.  Perceptual Behavior: Recurrence Analysis of a Haptic Exploratory Procedure , 2002, Perception.

[43]  W. Epstein,et al.  Perception of space and motion , 1995 .

[44]  Drew H. Abney,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Influence of Musical Groove on Postural Sway , 2015 .

[45]  M. Turvey,et al.  An ecological analysis of knowing by wielding. , 1989, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[46]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Stages of manual exploration in haptic object identification , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[47]  Raoul M Bongers,et al.  Variations of Tool and Task Characteristics Reveal That Tool-Use Postures Are Anticipated , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[48]  H. Solomon,et al.  Movement-produced invariants in haptic explorations: An example of a self-organizing, information-driven, intentional system , 1988 .

[49]  N. A. Bernstein Dexterity and Its Development , 1996 .

[50]  H. Haken,et al.  Synergetics of Cognition , 1990 .

[51]  M T Turvey,et al.  Perceiving the Sweet Spot , 1999, Perception.

[52]  M. Turvey Dynamic touch. , 1996, The American psychologist.

[53]  Michael F. Shlesinger,et al.  Dynamic patterns in complex systems , 1988 .

[54]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  Jeffrey B Wagman,et al.  Haptically creating affordances: the user-tool interface. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.