Statistical distribution of foveal transverse chromatic aberration, pupil centration, and angle psi in a population of young adult eyes.

Subjective transverse chromatic aberration (sTCA) manifest at the fovea was determined for a population of 85 young adults (19-38 years old) by means of a two-dimensional, two-color, vernier alignment technique. The statistical distribution of sTCA was well fitted by a bivariate Gaussian function with mean values that were not significantly different from zero in either the horizontal or the vertical direction. We conclude from this result that a hypothetical, average eye representing the population mean of human eyes with medium-sized pupils is free of foveal sTCA. However, the absolute magnitude of sTCA for any given individual was often significantly greater than zero and ranged from 0.05 to 2.67 arcmin for the red and the blue lights of a computer monitor (mean wavelengths, 605 and 497 nm, respectively). The statistical distribution of the absolute magnitude of sTCA was well described by a Rayleigh probability distribution with a mean of 0.8 arcmin. A simple device useful for population screening in a clinical setting was also tested and gave concordant results. Assuming that sTCA at the fovea is due to decentering of the pupil with respect to the visual axis, we infer from these results that the pupil is, on average, well centered in human eyes. The average magnitude of pupil decentration in individual eyes is less than 0.5 mm, which corresponds to psi = 3 deg for the angle between the achromatic and the visual axes of the eye.

[1]  P Simonet,et al.  Effect of illuminance on the directions of chromostereopsis and transverse chromatic aberration observed with natural pupils , 1990, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[2]  A Bradley,et al.  Effect of ocular chromatic aberration on monocular visual performance. , 1991, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[3]  H. E. Cross Ectopia lentis et pupillae. , 1979, American journal of ophthalmology.

[4]  H E Bedell,et al.  Magnitude of lateral chromatic aberration across the retina of the human eye. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[5]  P A Howarth,et al.  THE LATERAL CHROMATIC ABERRATION OF THE EYE , 1984, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[6]  J. Sundet,et al.  Effects of colour on perceived depth. Review of experiments and evaluation of theories. , 1978, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[7]  D. Birch,et al.  Inferred positive phototropic activity in human photoreceptors. , 1981, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[8]  J M Enoch,et al.  Monocular light exclusion for a period of days reduces directional sensitivity of the human retina. , 1979, Science.

[9]  J. J. Vos,et al.  The color stereoscopic effect. , 1966, Vision research.

[10]  R A Applegate,et al.  Induced movement of receptor alignment toward a new pupillary aperture. , 1981, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[11]  G. M. Hope,et al.  Directional sensitivity of the foveal and parafoveal retina. , 1973, Investigative ophthalmology.

[12]  Harold Metcalf Stiles-Crawford Apodization , 1965 .

[13]  A. Meeteren,et al.  Image quality of the human eye for eccentric entrance pupils , 1983, Vision Research.

[14]  J M Sundet The effect of pupil size variations on the colour stereoscopic phenomenon. , 1972, Vision research.

[15]  V. Lakshminarayanan,et al.  Effect of monocular light exclusion on the Stiles-Crawford function , 1987, Vision Research.

[16]  A. Bradley,et al.  The effect of pupil size on chromostereopsis and chromatic diplopia: Interaction between the Stiles-Crawford effect and chromatic aberrations , 1992, Vision Research.

[17]  L. Thibos Calculation of the influence of lateral chromatic aberration on image quality across the visual field. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[18]  B. N. Kishto,et al.  The colour stereoscopic effect. , 1965, Vision research.

[19]  A. Bradley,et al.  Theory and measurement of ocular chromatic aberration , 1990, Vision Research.

[20]  A. Bradley,et al.  The chromatic eye: a new reduced-eye model of ocular chromatic aberration in humans. , 1992, Applied optics.

[21]  F. H. Verhoeff An Optical Illusion Due to Chromatic Aberration , 1928 .

[22]  F. Campbell,et al.  The effect of chromatic aberration on visual acuity , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  Larry N. Thibos,et al.  Reversals of the colour-depth illusion explained by ocular chromatic aberration , 1995, Vision Research.

[24]  J. J. Vos Some New Aspects of Color Stereoscopy , 1960 .

[25]  Larry N. Thibos,et al.  Interocular differences in transverse chromatic aberration determine chromostereopsis for small pupils , 1991, Vision Research.

[26]  P Simonet,et al.  The Julius F. Neumueller Award in Optics, 1989: change of pupil centration with change of illumination and pupil size. , 1992, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[27]  A Bradley Glenn A. Fry Award Lecture 1991: perceptual manifestations of imperfect optics in the human eye: attempts to correct for ocular chromatic aberration. , 1992, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[28]  M. Campbell,et al.  The optical transverse chromatic aberration on the fovea of the human eye , 1990, Vision Research.

[29]  W. N. Charman,et al.  The effect of pupil centration and diameter on ocular performance , 1988, Vision Research.

[30]  R A Applegate,et al.  Parametric representation of Stiles-Crawford functions: normal variation of peak location and directionality. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[31]  J. Faubert Seeing depth in colour: More than just what meets the eyes , 1994, Vision Research.