Comparing Angular and Curved Shapes in Terms of Implicit Associations and Approach/Avoidance Responses

Most people prefer smoothly curved shapes over more angular shapes. We investigated the origin of this effect using abstract shapes and implicit measures of semantic association and preference. In Experiment 1 we used a multidimensional Implicit Association Test (IAT) to verify the strength of the association of curved and angular polygons with danger (safe vs. danger words), valence (positive vs. negative words) and gender (female vs. male names). Results showed that curved polygons were associated with safe and positive concepts and with female names, whereas angular polygons were associated with danger and negative concepts and with male names. Experiment 2 used a different implicit measure, which avoided any need to categorise the stimuli. Using a revised version of the Stimulus Response Compatibility (SRC) task we tested with a stick figure (i.e., the manikin) approach and avoidance reactions to curved and angular polygons. We found that RTs for approaching vs. avoiding angular polygons did not differ, even in the condition where the angles were more pronounced. By contrast participants were faster and more accurate when moving the manikin towards curved shapes. Experiment 2 suggests that preference for curvature cannot derive entirely from an association of angles with threat. We conclude that smoothly curved contours make these abstract shapes more pleasant. Further studies are needed to clarify the nature of such a preference.

[1]  David C. Zhu,et al.  Recognizing Threat: A Simple Geometric Shape Activates Neural Circuitry for Threat Detection , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Athayde Ribeiro da Silva A model for intelligence , 1984 .

[3]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  On the generality of the affective Simon effect , 2001 .

[4]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[5]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[6]  Marco Bertamini,et al.  Implicit Association of Symmetry with Positive Valence, High Arousal and Simplicity , 2013 .

[7]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  The Implicit Association Test at Age 7: A Methodological and Conceptual Review , 2007 .

[8]  W. Gerbino,et al.  Sound-shape congruency affects the multisensory response enhancement , 2010 .

[9]  Christine L Larson,et al.  The shape of threat: simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. , 2007, Emotion.

[10]  Tamara N. Gheorghes,et al.  Do observers like curvature or do they dislike angularity? , 2015, British journal of psychology.

[11]  W. Köhler Gestalt psychology , 1967 .

[12]  Claus-Christian Carbon,et al.  Dimensions in appreciation of car interior design , 2005 .

[13]  G. Carrus,et al.  The Automatic Aesthetic Evaluation of Different Art and Architectural Styles , 2011 .

[14]  R. de Raedt,et al.  Being Moved , 2010, Psychological science.

[15]  M. Bar,et al.  Humans Prefer Curved Visual Objects , 2006, Psychological science.

[16]  Paul J. Silvia,et al.  Do People Prefer Curved Objects? Angularity, Expertise, and Aesthetic Preference , 2009 .

[17]  Sophie M. Wuerger,et al.  The IAT shows no evidence for Kandinsky's color-shape associations , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[18]  William Hogarth,et al.  Analysis of Beauty, Written with a View of Fixing the Fluctuating Ideas of Taste , 2017, Eighteenth-Century British Aesthetics.

[19]  A. Gomila,et al.  Is the human initial preference for rounded shapes universal? Preliminary results of an ongoing cross-cultural research , 2013 .

[20]  Moshe Bar,et al.  Emotional Valence Modulates the Preference for Curved Objects , 2011, Perception.

[21]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Marco Bertamini,et al.  Implicit affective evaluation of visual symmetry. , 2012, Emotion.

[23]  M. Bar,et al.  Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdala activation , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  H. Eysenck,et al.  A Model for intelligence , 1982 .

[25]  Oshin Vartanian,et al.  Impact of contour on aesthetic judgments and approach-avoidance decisions in architecture , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  Perceptual organization of complex visual configurations by young infants , 1997 .

[27]  M. Sääksjärvi,et al.  Extending the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Assessing Consumer Attitudes Based on Multi-Dimensional Implicit Associations , 2011, PloS one.

[28]  E. Milán,et al.  The Kiki-Bouba Effect A Case of Personification and Ideaesthesia , 2013 .

[29]  John B. Carroll,et al.  Ability and Task Difficulty in Cognitive Psychology , 1981 .