Age Diminishes Performance on an Antisaccade Eye Movement Task

Tthe antisaccade eye movement task, which has been linked to frontal lobe function, presents a target in one visual field and asks subjects to move their eyes to the same location in the opposite field. The task requires inhibition of the reflexive prosaccade to the cue, initiation of the antisaccade to the opposite field, and visuo-spatial memory of the cue location. Forty-two subjects from 19-79 years of age performed this task and a control task, visually guided saccades to the cue itself, to determine which functions are affected by aging. The time to initiate antisaccades increased linearly with age at a rate greater than the time to initiate visually guided saccades. This difference suggests that the processing time to inhibit the incorrect movement to the cue is selectively increased with age. Older subjects also made more incorrect prosaccadic movements to the cue, a finding consistent with the loss of inhibitory processing capacity. The accuracy of movements did not change, which suggests that visuo-spatial memory is unaffected by aging.

[1]  D. Levy,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  A. Diamond Developmental Time Course in Human Infants and Infant Monkeys, and the Neural Bases of, Inhibitory Control in Reaching a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Adèle,et al.  The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Functions. A conference. May 20-24, 1989, Philadelphia, Pa. , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  B. Horwitz,et al.  Interregional correlations of resting cerebral glucose metabolism in old and young women , 1992, Brain Research.

[6]  R. DeTeresa,et al.  Neocortical cell counts in normal human adult aging , 1987, Annals of neurology.

[7]  R W Baloh,et al.  Effect of aging on eye tracking. , 1980, Archives of neurology.

[8]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task , 1980, Vision Research.

[9]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[10]  R. Roberts,et al.  Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. , 1994 .

[11]  J. Botwinick,et al.  Sensory-set factors in age differences in reaction time. , 1971, The Journal of Genetic Psychology.

[12]  P. Huttenlocher Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. , 1979, Brain research.

[13]  R. E. Dustman,et al.  Memory-For-Designs Test: comparison of performance of young and old adults. , 1980, Journal of clinical psychology.

[14]  J. Sharpe,et al.  Senescent saccades. Effects of aging on their accuracy, latency and velocity. , 1987, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[15]  Daniel W. Hommer,et al.  A quantitative analysis of saccades and smooth pursuit during visual pursuit tracking A comparison of schizophrenics with normals and substance abusing controls , 1992, Schizophrenia Research.

[16]  Robert L. Heilbronner,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, Second Edition , 1989 .

[17]  H. Werner,et al.  Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. , 1962, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[18]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[19]  W. Slidders,et al.  The effect of advanced old age on the neurone content of the cerebral cortex Observations with an automatic image analyser point counting method , 1983, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.