Generic Statements Require Little Evidence for Acceptance but Have Powerful Implications

Generic statements (e.g., "Birds lay eggs") express generalizations about categories. In this paper, we hypothesized that there is a paradoxical asymmetry at the core of generic meaning, such that these sentences have extremely strong implications but require little evidence to be judged true. Four experiments confirmed the hypothesized asymmetry: Participants interpreted novel generics such as "Lorches have purple feathers" as referring to nearly all lorches, but they judged the same novel generics to be true given a wide range of prevalence levels (e.g., even when only 10% or 30% of lorches had purple feathers). A second hypothesis, also confirmed by the results, was that novel generic sentences about dangerous or distinctive properties would be more acceptable than generic sentences that were similar but did not have these connotations. In addition to clarifying important aspects of generics' meaning, these findings are applicable to a range of real-world processes such as stereotyping and political discourse.

[1]  Marjorie Rhodes,et al.  Sample diversity and premise typicality in inductive reasoning: Evidence for developmental change , 2008, Cognition.

[2]  James A. Hampton,et al.  The inverse conjunction fallacy , 2006 .

[3]  Östen Dahl Formal semantics of Natural Language: On generics , 1975 .

[4]  H. Wellman,et al.  Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious , 1991, Cognition.

[5]  S. Gelman,et al.  Beyond labeling: the role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. , 1991, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[6]  John M. Lawler,et al.  Studies in English generics , 1973 .

[7]  Joel R. Levin,et al.  A controlled, powerful multiple-comparison strategy for several situations. , 1994 .

[8]  S. Gelman Learning from others: children's construction of concepts. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  Sandeep Prasada,et al.  Principled and statistical connections in common sense conception , 2006, Cognition.

[10]  Amanda C. Brandone,et al.  Theory-based considerations influence the interpretation of generic sentences , 2010, Language and cognitive processes.

[11]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[12]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[13]  David E. Kanouse,et al.  Subjective Acceptance of Verbal Generalizations , 1966 .

[14]  D. Dawda,et al.  AND COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT , 2006 .

[15]  P. Harris,et al.  Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements , 2004, Psychological science.

[16]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  Preschool children’s use of cues to generic meaning , 2008, Cognition.

[17]  Gregory Norman Carlson,et al.  Reference to kinds in English , 1977 .

[18]  S. Gelman,et al.  The Essential Child : Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought , 2003 .

[19]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  The impact of generic language about ability on children's achievement motivation. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[20]  A. Markman,et al.  Inference using categories. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  J. Deloache,et al.  Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. , 2010, Developmental science.

[22]  R. Abelson,et al.  THE SUBJECTIVE USE OF INDUCTIVE EVIDENCE. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  D. C. Howell Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd ed. , 1992 .

[24]  Marianne G. Taylor,et al.  Mother-child conversations about gender: Understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs: I. Introduction. , 2004 .

[25]  RENAAT DECLERCK,et al.  The origins of genericity , 1991 .

[26]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  Information learned from generic language becomes central to children’s biological concepts: Evidence from their open-ended explanations , 2009, Cognition.

[27]  Barbara W Sarnecka,et al.  Generic Language in Parent-Child Conversations , 2008, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[28]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[29]  Sarah-Jane Leslie,et al.  Generics, Prevalence, and Default Inferences , 2009 .

[30]  Sarah-Jane Leslie,et al.  Do ducks lay eggs? How people interpret generic assertions , 2007 .

[31]  C. Sumiyoshi CATEGORY BASED INDUCTION , 1997 .

[32]  S. Gelman The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  Marianne G. Taylor,et al.  Mother-child conversations about gender : understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs , 2004 .

[34]  L. Rips Inductive judgments about natural categories. , 1975 .

[35]  V. Jaswal,et al.  Adults Don't Always Know Best , 2006, Psychological science.

[36]  George S. Cree,et al.  Distinctive features hold a privileged status in the computation of word meaning: Implications for theories of semantic memory. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  Sarah-Jane Leslie,et al.  Generics: Cognition and Acquisition , 2008 .

[38]  S. Sloman Feature-Based Induction , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  G. Carlson,et al.  1 Truth-Conditions of Generic Sentences : Two Contrasting Views , 1988 .

[40]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Learning Words for Kinds: Generic Noun Phrases in Acquisition. , 2004 .

[41]  Vanessa Lobue,et al.  More than just another face in the crowd: superior detection of threatening facial expressions in children and adults. , 2009, Developmental science.

[42]  Sandeep Prasada,et al.  Acquiring generic knowledge , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[44]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[45]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  Francis Jeffry Pelletier,et al.  The Generic book , 1997 .

[47]  D. Medin,et al.  The role of theories in conceptual coherence. , 1985, Psychological review.

[48]  G. Gazdar,et al.  Formal semantics of natural language: papers from a colloquium sponsored by the King's College Research Centre, Cambridge Ed. by Edward L. Keenan (review) , 2015 .

[49]  Renaat Declerck,et al.  The manifold interpretations of generic sentences , 1986 .

[50]  Jon R. Star,et al.  Children's Use of Generics in Inductive Inferences , 2002 .

[51]  Takashi Yamauchi,et al.  Labeling bias and categorical induction: generative aspects of category information. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Developmental changes in the understanding of generics , 2007, Cognition.

[53]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[54]  G. Glass,et al.  Consequences of Failure to Meet Assumptions Underlying the Fixed Effects Analyses of Variance and Covariance , 1972 .

[55]  A. Tversky,et al.  Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment , 1983 .