The influence of environmental features on route selection in an emergency situation.

Understanding the influence of external information at a lower level of awareness during the processes of route selection could be a key factor to predict user's movements within complex buildings, avoiding wayfinding problems and improving egress in emergency situations. This study aims to verify whether corridor intersection configuration attributes, such as width and brightness, act as factors of attraction to improve the affordance of indoor hallways during an emergency egress situation, using a VR-based methodology. The main hypotheses are that users tend to move along either, wider or brighter corridors. Thirty volunteers participated in this study, moving along 57 different corridors, according to the experimental conditions of the study. The results suggest that people prefer to follow brighter pathways in "T-type" and "F-type" intersections, and wider corridors in "T-type" intersections. In situations where these variables are in conflict, there is a preference for brighter paths in both intersection configurations.

[1]  F. edridge-green Tests for Colour-Blindness , 1895, Nature.

[2]  Markus Knauff,et al.  Finding the Way Inside: Linking Architectural Design Analysis and Cognitive Processes , 2004, Spatial Cognition.

[3]  Francisco Rebelo,et al.  Virtual Reality in Wayfinding Studies , 2010 .

[4]  Michael K. McBeath,et al.  Right-Handers and Americans Favor Turning to the Right , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[5]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Using desktop virtual environments to investigate the role of landmarks , 2002, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[6]  Justin B. Rousek,et al.  The use of simulated visual impairment to identify hospital design elements that contribute to wayfinding difficulties , 2011 .

[7]  T. Stoffregen Affordances as Properties of the Animal-Environment System , 2003, How Shall Affordances be Refined? Four Perspectives.

[8]  Francisco Rebelo,et al.  Are People Able to Develop Cognitive Maps of Virtual Environments While Performing a Way Finding Task , 2010 .

[9]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Wayfinding Performance in and the Spatial Knowledge of a Color-coded Building for Adults and Children , 2004, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[10]  Michael O'Neill,et al.  Effects of Signage and Floor Plan Configuration on Wayfinding Accuracy , 1991 .

[11]  R. Dalton The Secret Is To Follow Your Nose , 2001 .

[12]  Francisco Rebelo,et al.  Indoor Human Wayfinding Performance Using Vertical and Horizontal Signage in Virtual Reality , 2014 .

[13]  T Landis,et al.  Opposite turning behavior in right-handers and non-right-handers suggests a link between handedness and cerebral dopamine asymmetries. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[14]  J. Chébat,et al.  Lost in a mall, the effects of gender, familiarity with the shopping mall and the shopping values on shoppers' wayfinding processes , 2005 .

[15]  Ernesto Filgueiras,et al.  Human Interaction Data Acquisition Software for Virtual Reality: A User-Centered Design Approach , 2010 .

[16]  Edwin R. Galea,et al.  Experimental analysis of the effectiveness of emergency signage and its implementation in evacuation simulation , 2012 .

[17]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[18]  Ruth Alison Conroy-Dalton,et al.  Spatial navigation in immersive virtual environments , 2001 .

[19]  Thomas J. Pingel,et al.  Strategic Elements of Route Choice , 2010 .

[20]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[21]  M. Turvey Affordances and Prospective Control: An Outline of the Ontology , 1992 .

[22]  R. Cohen,et al.  The Representation of Landmarks and Routes. , 1980 .

[23]  Michael O'Neill,et al.  Evaluation of a Conceptual Model of Architectural Legibility , 1991 .

[24]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[25]  Luciano Gamberini,et al.  Exploring the Suitability of Virtual Environments for Safety Training: Signals, Norms and Ambiguity in a Simulated Emergency Escape , 2001, Cognition, Technology & Work.

[26]  Kemal Yıldırım,et al.  The effects of color and light on indoor wayfinding and the evaluation of the perceived environment , 2012 .

[27]  D. Butler,et al.  Wayfinding by Newcomers in a Complex Building , 1993 .

[28]  P. Wilson,et al.  Orientation-Free Representations from Navigation through a Computer-Simulated Environment , 1996 .

[29]  Stanley Coren,et al.  The Left-Hander Syndrome: The Causes and Consequences of Left-Handedness , 1992 .

[30]  Colin Potts,et al.  Design of Everyday Things , 1988 .

[31]  M S Hallbeck,et al.  Improving and analyzing signage within a healthcare setting. , 2011, Applied ergonomics.

[32]  Itzhak Omer,et al.  The implications of inter-visibility between landmarks on wayfinding performance: An investigation using a virtual urban environment , 2007, Comput. Environ. Urban Syst..

[33]  I. Gordon Theories of Visual Perception , 1989 .

[34]  Jack L. Nasar Environmental Factors, Perceived Distance and Spatial Behavior , 1983 .

[35]  Beatrix Emo,et al.  Wayfinding and Spatial Configuration: evidencefrom street corners , 2012 .

[36]  Lyle H. Taylor,et al.  MOVEMENT OF PEOPLE TOWARD LIGHTS , 1974 .

[37]  Romedi Passini,et al.  Wayfinding: People, Signs, and Architecture , 1992 .

[38]  E. S. Robinson,et al.  The Psychology of Public Education. , 1933, American journal of public health and the nation's health.

[39]  John H. Bailey,et al.  Virtual spaces and real world places: transfer of route knowledge , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..