Structure and Usage of the Vocal Repertoire of Callithrix jacchus

We recorded the vocal repertoire and behavior of individuals from 3 groups of wild common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) via focal sampling and ad libitum sampling in a fragment of Atlantic Rain Forest in Pernambuco, northeast Brazil. Our aims were: 1) to provide a detailed quantitative description of the vocalizations and vocal repertoire of common marmosets in the wild, and to compare the repertoire with that described from conspecific captives; 2) to investigate if differences exist in vocal repertoire between wild individuals of different ages; and 3) to explore the behavioral contexts associated with specific vocalizations in free-living Callithrix jacchus. This is the first study describing common marmoset calls in the wild and it shows that their vocal repertoire comprises 13 different calls. Though wild and captive calls were similar, differences occurred in the form of the alarm calls given for different potential predators, the tsê call, and very brief whistle. In addition, the tsee call did not occur in wild individuals and wild infants did not utter the twitter call. The age of the subjects influenced the call types present in the vocal repertoire, e.g., the loud cry was specific to infants, whereas only adults gave alarm calls. The behavior of both the caller and the potential receivers seemed to affect the frequency of wild common marmoset calls. One could predict intricate vocalization-based communications for the system based on their relatively complex social organization and densely leafed arboreal habitat. Our study confirms the prediction.

[1]  Kurt Hammerschmidt,et al.  Functionally Referential Alarm Calls in Tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) – Evidence from Playback Experiments , 2006 .

[2]  N. Masataka,et al.  Cross‐Sectional and Longitudinal Studies of the Development of Group Differences in Acoustic Features of Coo Calls in Two Groups of Japanese Macaques , 2006 .

[3]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Acoustic analysis of vocal development in a New World primate, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  Philipp Goedeking,et al.  Primate Vocal Communication , 1988, Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

[5]  P. D. Marsden A Primatologia no Brasil , 1985 .

[6]  W. Maier,et al.  Field observations on Callithrix jacchus jacchus L , 1981 .

[7]  J. Lamprecht Biologische Forschung: Von der Planung bis zur Publikation , 1992 .

[8]  J. Gros-Louis Contexts and behavioral correlates of trill vocalizations in wild white‐faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) , 2002, American journal of primatology.

[9]  E. Zimmermann,et al.  Colony specificity in a social call of mouse lemurs (Microcebus ssp.) , 2001, American journal of primatology.

[10]  P. C. Lee How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species , 1990 .

[11]  V. Boere,et al.  Reactions to Potential Predators in Captive-Born Marmosets (Callithrix penicillata) , 2002, International Journal of Primatology.

[12]  W. Fitch,et al.  Responses to natural and synthetic phee calls by common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 1994, American journal of primatology.

[13]  J. Newman,et al.  Context and gender‐specific differences in the acoustic structure of common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) phee calls , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[14]  C. Ades,et al.  Long-distance calls in Neotropical primates. , 2004, Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciencias.

[15]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.

[16]  J. R. Napier,et al.  The Natural History Of The Primates , 1985 .

[17]  R. Mittermeier,et al.  Ecology and behavior of neotropical primates , 1981 .

[18]  J. Newman,et al.  Squirrel monkey chuck call: vocal response to playback chucks based on acoustic structure and affiliative relationship with the caller , 2002, American journal of primatology.

[19]  L. Huber,et al.  Saltatory Search in Free-Living Callithrix jacchus: Environmental and Age Influences , 2007, International Journal of Primatology.

[20]  Clive K. Catchpole,et al.  The stability of the vocal signature in phee calls of the common marmoset, Callithrix jacchus , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[21]  W. Rice ANALYZING TABLES OF STATISTICAL TESTS , 1989, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[22]  S. Harris,et al.  STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION OF RED FOX VULPES VULPES VOCALISATIONS , 1993 .

[23]  M. Hauser,et al.  The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments. , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.

[24]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication , 1995, Springer US.

[25]  C. Snowdon Communications as Social Interaction: Its Importance in Ontogeny and Adult Behavior , 1988 .

[26]  B. Bezerra,et al.  Infanticide and cannibalism in a free‐ranging plurally breeding group of common marmosets (Callithrix Jacchus) , 2007, American journal of primatology.

[27]  C. Snowdon,et al.  'BABBLING' IN PYGMY MARMOSETS: DEVELOPMENT AFTER INFANCY , 2001 .

[28]  Charles T. Snowdon,et al.  The Complex Vocal Repertoire of the Adult Cotton‐top Tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus)1) , 2010 .

[29]  Trevor B. Poole,et al.  An ethogram of the common marmoset (Calithrix jacchus jacchus): General behavioural repertoire , 1976, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[31]  S. Altmann,et al.  Social communication among primates , 1967 .

[32]  J. Newman,et al.  Context and sex differences exist in the acoustic structure of phee calls by newly–paired common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 1999, American journal of primatology.

[33]  V. Boere Environmental enrichment for neotropical primates in captivity , 2001 .

[34]  C. Snowdon Social processes in communication and cognition in callitrichid monkeys: a review , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[35]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Acoustic adaptation in pygmy marmoset contact calls: Locational cues vary with distances between conspecifics , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[36]  G. Jones,et al.  Predation by the tayra on the common marmoset and the pale-throated three-toed sloth , 2008, Journal of Ethology.

[37]  Leslie J. Digby,et al.  Infant care, infanticide, and female reproductive strategies in polygynous groups of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 1995, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[38]  Tessa E. Smith Individual olfactory signatures in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 2006, American journal of primatology.

[39]  A. Whiten,et al.  Primate Calls, Human Language, and Nonverbal Communication [and Comments and Reply] , 1993, Current Anthropology.

[40]  P. K. Seth,et al.  Perspectives in primate biology , 1983 .

[41]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  The auditory behaviour of primates: a neuroethological perspective , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  THE FUNCTIONAL SIGNIFICANCE OF DIANA MONKEY 'CLEAR' CALLS , 2001 .

[43]  G. Epple,et al.  Comparative studies on vocalization in marmoset monkeys (Hapalidae). , 1968, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[44]  L. Huber,et al.  Social influences on the development of foraging behavior in free‐living common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 2006, American journal of primatology.

[45]  M. Maurus,et al.  A New Approach to Finding Components Essential for Intraspecific Communication , 1988 .

[46]  H. Buchanan-Smith,et al.  Vigilance in Single- and Mixed-Species Groups of Tamarins (Saguinus labiatus and Saguinus fuscicollis) , 1997, International Journal of Primatology.

[47]  S. Ferrari,et al.  A re-evaluation of the social organisation of the Callitrichidae, with reference to the ecological differences between genera. , 1989, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[48]  R. Howard Principles of Animal Communication , 1999 .

[49]  S. Savage-Rumbaugh,et al.  Vocal Production by a Language-Competent Pan paniscus , 2003, International Journal of Primatology.

[50]  A. R. M. Pontes,et al.  Sleeping sites of common marmosets ( Callithrix jacchus ) in defaunated urban forest fragments: a strategy to maximize food intake , 2005 .

[51]  Michael J. Owren,et al.  Some Analysis Methods That May be Useful to Acoustic Primatologists , 1995 .

[52]  Can wild common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) solve the parallel strings task? , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[53]  M. Mcphee Effects of Captivity on Response to a Novel Environment in the Oldfield Mouse (Peromyscus polionotus subgriseus) , 2003, International Journal of Comparative Psychology.

[54]  Aimee F. Campbell,et al.  Use of Trill Vocalizations To Coordinate Troop Movement Among White-Faced Capuchins: a Second Field Test , 1995 .

[55]  J. Itani Vocal communication of the wild Japanese monkey , 1963, Primates.

[56]  C. Snowdon,et al.  The effects of social status on food-associated calling behaviour in captive cotton-top tamarins , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[57]  M. F. Stevenson,et al.  The marmosets genus Callithrix , 1988 .