The Effect of Legal Expert Commentary on Lay Judgments of Judicial Decision Making

The public's view of the judiciary is a key factor in the legitimacy of any legal system. Ideally, popular judgments of the adjudicative branch would be independent of the outcomes of the decisions it furnishes. In a previous study (Simon & Scurich 2011), we found that lay people's evaluations of the judicial decision‐making process were highly contingent on the decision outcomes. Participants gave favorable evaluations of the judges and their decisions when they agreed with the judges' outcomes, but reported negative evaluations when they disagreed with them. These results held true across four different types of judicial reasoning, and despite the fact that all decisions were described as having followed proper procedures and been argued by competent lawyers. That study left open the possibility that the public's judgments might be moderated by professional elites, namely, legal experts. Indeed, in real life, much of the public's information about judicial decisions is derived from legal experts who communicate and comment on them in the media. This study examined the effect of professional commentators on lay people's judgments of judicial decision making. We found that the experts' commentaries do not alter participants' evaluations of the courts' decisions, as the evaluations continue to be influenced strongly by the participants' agreement with the outcomes of the judges' decisions. Moreover, lay people's reactions to the experts follow a similar pattern: the experts are deemed competent and their commentaries are deemed reliable when the participants agree with the outcomes propounded by the experts, but the opposite is true when the participants' preferred outcomes are incongruent with the outcomes endorsed by the experts. These findings suggest that the outcome‐dominated judgments of courts cannot easily be tempered by professional elites. This conclusion could also provide some insight into the dynamic process that enables political polarization.

[1]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  Can We Trust Intuitive Jurors? An Experimental Analysis , 2008 .

[2]  Nicholas Scurich,et al.  Lay Judgments of Judicial Decision-Making , 2011 .

[3]  Daniel L. Sherrell,et al.  Source effects in communication and persuasion research: A meta-analysis of effect size , 1993 .

[4]  J. Koehler The Influence of Prior Beliefs on Scientific Judgments of Evidence Quality , 1993 .

[5]  Dieter Frey,et al.  Recent Research on Selective Exposure to Information , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[6]  H. Kelley,et al.  Communication and Persuasion: Psychological Studies of Opinion Change , 1982 .

[7]  S. Sherman,et al.  Attitude bolstering when behavior is inconsistent with central attitudes , 1980 .

[8]  Diana C. Mutz,et al.  Facilitating Communication across Lines of Political Difference: The Role of Mass Media , 2001, American Political Science Review.

[9]  T. Tyler,et al.  Why People Obey the Law , 2021 .

[10]  A. Glöckner,et al.  Can We Trust Intuitive Jurors? Standards of Proof and the Probative Value of Evidence in Coherence‐Based Reasoning , 2013 .

[11]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Source factors and the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion , 1984 .

[12]  Andrei Marmor Law in the age of pluralism , 2007 .

[13]  S. Read,et al.  Bias at the Racetrack: Effects of Individual Expertise and Task Importance on Predecision Reevaluation of Alternatives , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[14]  Nicholas Scurich,et al.  Lay Judgments of Judicial Decision Making , 2011 .

[15]  Kenneth I. Winston,et al.  The Forms and Limits of Adjudication , 1978 .

[16]  T. Tyler Why People Obey the Law , 2021 .

[17]  Paul D. Sweeney,et al.  Selective exposure: Voter information preferences and the Watergate affair. , 1984 .

[18]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[19]  Janice Nadler Flouting the Law , 2005 .

[20]  S. Breyer Making Our Democracy Work: A Judge's View , 2010 .

[21]  J. Krosnick,et al.  Attitude centrality, organization, and measurement. , 1982 .

[22]  A. Nanjundappa,et al.  Do the means justify the ends? , 2009, Expert review of cardiovascular therapy.

[23]  Dennis Chong Rational Lives: Norms and Values in Politics and Society , 2000 .

[24]  Matthew A. Baum,et al.  Shot by the Messenger: Partisan Cues and Public Opinion Regarding National Security and War , 2009 .

[25]  R. Zane The legal process. , 2009, Emergency medicine clinics of North America.

[26]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Bidirectional reasoning in decision making by constraint satisfaction , 1999 .

[27]  K J Holyoak,et al.  The Emergence of Coherence Over the Course of Decision Making , 2004 .

[28]  Alexander M. Bickel The Least Dangerous Branch: The Supreme Court at the Bar of Politics , 1962 .

[29]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[30]  Duncan Kennedy,et al.  Freedom and Constraint in Adjudication: A Critical Phenomenology. , 1986 .

[31]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Instructional Manipulation Checks: Detecting Satisficing to Increase Statistical Power , 2009 .

[32]  Robert S. Wyer,et al.  The effects of feedback about self and others on the recall and judgments of feedback-relevant information ☆ , 1983 .

[33]  Elizabeth Mullen,et al.  Moral Spillovers: The Effect of Moral Violations on Deviant Behavior , 2008 .

[34]  J. Darley,et al.  Justice, Liability, And Blame: Community Views And The Criminal Law , 1995 .

[35]  J. Waldron,et al.  Thoughtfulness and the Rule of Law , 2011 .

[36]  S. Read,et al.  4. Parallel constraint satisfaction as a mechanism for cognitive consistency , 2012 .

[37]  John G. Bullock Elite Influence on Public Opinion in an Informed Electorate , 2011, American Political Science Review.

[38]  L. Skitka Do the Means Always Justify the Ends, or Do the Ends Sometimes Justify the Means? A Value Protection Model of Justice Reasoning , 2002 .

[39]  S. Schulz-Hardt,et al.  Confirmation bias in sequential information search after preliminary decisions: an expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective exposure to information. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  H. M. Hart,et al.  The legal process : basic problems in the making and application of law , 1958 .

[41]  C. Sunstein Republic.com 2.0 , 2007 .

[42]  S. Read,et al.  The redux of cognitive consistency theories: evidence judgments by constraint satisfaction. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  Gregory A. Caldeira,et al.  Has Legal Realism Damaged the Legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court , 2009 .

[44]  Angela Fagerlin,et al.  Biased Assimilation of Sociopolitical Arguments: Evaluating the 1996 U.S. Presidential Debate , 2002 .

[45]  R. Dworkin Law's Empire , 1987 .

[46]  Philip P. Frickey,et al.  THE MAKING OF THE LEGAL PROCESS , 1994 .

[47]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  A. Glöckner,et al.  Coherence Shifts in Probabilistic Inference Tasks , 2009 .