Repeated Exposure to Illusory Sense of Body Ownership and Agency Over a Moving Virtual Body Improves Executive Functioning and Increases Prefrontal Cortex Activity in the Elderly

We previously showed that the illusory sense of ownership and agency over a moving body in immersive virtual reality (displayed in a first-person perspective) can trigger subjective and physiological reactions on the real subject’s body and, therefore, an acute improvement of cognitive functions after a single session of high-intensity intermittent exercise performed exclusively by one’s own virtual body, similar to what happens when we actually do physical activity. As well as confirming previous results, here, we aimed at finding in the elderly an increased improvement after a longer virtual training with similar characteristics. Forty-two healthy older subjects (28 females, average age = 71.71 years) completed a parallel-group randomized controlled trial (RCT; UMIN000039843, umin.ac.jp) including an adapted version of the virtual training previously used: while sitting, participants observed the virtual body in a first-person perspective (1PP) or a third-person perspective (3PP) performing 20 min of virtual high-intensity intermittent exercise (vHIE; the avatar switched between fast and slow walking every 2 min). This was repeated twice a week for 6 weeks. During the vHIE, we measured the heart rate and administered questionnaires to evaluate illusory body ownership and agency. Before the beginning of the intervention, immediately after the first session of vHIE, and at the end of the entire intervention, we evaluated the cognitive performance at the Stroop task with online recording of the hemodynamic activity over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. While we confirm previous results regarding the virtual illusion and its physiological effects, we did not find significant cognitive or neural improvement immediately after the first vHIE session. As a novelty, in the 1PP group only, we detected a significant decrease in the response time of the Stroop task in the post-intervention assessment compared to its baseline; coherently, we found an increased activation on left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) after the entire intervention. While the current results strengthen the impact of the virtual full-body illusion and its physiological consequences on the elderly as well, they might have stronger and more established body representations. Perhaps, a longer and increased exposure to those illusions is necessary to initiate the cascade of events that culminates to an improved cognitive performance.

[1]  A. Yasumura,et al.  The acute effect of moderate-intensity exercise on inhibitory control and activation of prefrontal cortex in younger and older adults , 2021, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  W. Teramoto,et al.  Multisensory integration involved in the body perception of community-dwelling older adults , 2021, Scientific reports.

[3]  W. Kunde,et al.  How to lose a hand: Sensory updating drives disembodiment , 2020, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[4]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Being the victim of virtual abuse changes default mode network responses to emotional expressions , 2020, Cortex.

[5]  Jaron Lanier,et al.  The Rocketbox Library and the Utility of Freely Available Rigged Avatars , 2020, Frontiers in Virtual Reality.

[6]  Ryuta Kawashima,et al.  Virtual training leads to physical, cognitive and neural benefits in healthy adults , 2020, NeuroImage.

[7]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Immersive virtual reality as a pedagogical tool in education: a systematic literature review of quantitative learning outcomes and experimental design , 2020, Journal of Computers in Education.

[8]  K. Delbaere,et al.  The effects of cognitive-motor training interventions on executive functions in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis , 2020, European Review of Aging and Physical Activity.

[9]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Being the Victim of Intimate Partner Violence in Virtual Reality: First- Versus Third-Person Perspective , 2020, Frontiers in Psychology.

[10]  Mel Slater,et al.  Which Body Would You Like to Have? The Impact of Embodied Perspective on Body Perception and Body Evaluation in Immersive Virtual Reality , 2020, Frontiers in Robotics and AI.

[11]  W. Hsu,et al.  Using virtual reality-based training to improve cognitive function, instrumental activities of daily living and neural efficiency in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. , 2020, European journal of physical and rehabilitation medicine.

[12]  Mel Slater,et al.  Manipulating the Perceived Shape and Color of a Virtual Limb Can Modulate Pain Responses , 2020, Journal of clinical medicine.

[13]  D. Burin,et al.  Relationships Between Personality Features and the Rubber Hand Illusion: An Exploratory Study , 2019, Front. Psychol..

[14]  L. Zapparoli,et al.  Neurofunctional correlates of body-ownership and sense of agency: A meta-analytical account of self-consciousness , 2019, Cortex.

[15]  Wei-Chun Hsu,et al.  Effects of Virtual Reality-Based Physical and Cognitive Training on Executive Function and Dual-Task Gait Performance in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Randomized Control Trial , 2019, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[16]  Mel Slater,et al.  It feels real: physiological responses to a stressful virtual reality environment and its impact on working memory , 2019, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[17]  Carlotta Fossataro,et al.  Hand blink reflex in virtual reality: The role of vision and proprioception in modulating defensive responses , 2020, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  Hajime Asama,et al.  Improvement of Sense of Agency During Upper-Limb Movement for Motor Rehabilitation Using Virtual Reality , 2019, 2019 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[19]  M. Pyasik,et al.  The Role of Body-Related Afferent Signals in Human Sense of Agency , 2019, Journal of experimental neuroscience.

[20]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Virtual training leads to real acute physical, cognitive, and neural benefits on healthy adults: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial , 2019, Trials.

[21]  Neil Burgess,et al.  A mechanistic account of bodily resonance and implicit bias , 2019, Cognition.

[22]  T. Wolbers,et al.  Preserved multisensory body representations in advanced age , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[23]  Mel Slater,et al.  Body ownership increases the interference between observed and executed movements , 2019, PloS one.

[24]  M. Zampini,et al.  Age-related changes in the sense of body ownership: New insights from the rubber hand illusion , 2018, PloS one.

[25]  P. Dal Lago,et al.  Can RR intervals editing and selection techniques interfere with the analysis of heart rate variability? , 2018, Brazilian journal of physical therapy.

[26]  M. Slater Immersion and the illusion of presence in virtual reality. , 2018, British journal of psychology.

[27]  Ryuta Kawashima,et al.  The My Active and Healthy Aging (My-AHA) ICT platform to detect and prevent frailty in older adults: Randomized control trial design and protocol , 2018, Alzheimer's & dementia.

[28]  Mel Slater,et al.  Virtually Being Einstein Results in an Improvement in Cognitive Task Performance and a Decrease in Age Bias , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[29]  K. Okazaki,et al.  Effects of 2-Year Cognitive–Motor Dual-Task Training on Cognitive Function and Motor Ability in Healthy Elderly People: A Pilot Study , 2018, Brain sciences.

[30]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  The Role of Age on Multisensory Bodily Experience: An Experimental Study with a Virtual Reality Full-Body Illusion , 2018, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[31]  Changchun Liu,et al.  Does the Temporal Asymmetry of Short-Term Heart Rate Variability Change during Regular Walking? A Pilot Study of Healthy Young Subjects , 2018, Comput. Math. Methods Medicine.

[32]  M. Pyasik,et al.  On the relation between body ownership and sense of agency: A link at the level of sensory-related signals. , 2018, Acta psychologica.

[33]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  A transferable high-intensity intermittent exercise improves executive performance in association with dorsolateral prefrontal activation in young adults , 2018, NeuroImage.

[34]  D. Zeller,et al.  Spatial attention and the malleability of bodily self in the elderly , 2018, Consciousness and Cognition.

[35]  Irene Ronga,et al.  “As long as that is my hand, that willed action is mine”: Timing of agency triggered by body ownership , 2018, Consciousness and Cognition.

[36]  J. R. Bacha,et al.  Effects of Kinect Adventures Games Versus Conventional Physical Therapy on Postural Control in Elderly People: A Randomized Controlled Trial. , 2017, Games for health journal.

[37]  E. Juratovac,et al.  Age-Related Changes , 2017 .

[38]  T. Wolbers,et al.  Embodiment in the aging mind , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  Roberto Llorens,et al.  Subjective, behavioral, and physiological responses to the rubber hand illusion do not vary with age in the adult phase , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  D. Burin,et al.  That’s my hand! Therefore, that’s my willed action: How body ownership acts upon conscious awareness of willed actions , 2017, Cognition.

[41]  J. Kállai,et al.  Multisensory integration and age-dependent sensitivity to body representation modification induced by the rubber hand illusion , 2017, Cognitive Processing.

[42]  J. Laks,et al.  Acute effects of exergames on cognitive function of institutionalized older persons: a single-blinded, randomized and controlled pilot study , 2017, Aging Clinical and Experimental Research.

[43]  Knut Engedal,et al.  Virtual Reality-Based Physical Exercise With Exergames (PhysEx) Improves Mental and Physical Health of Institutionalized Older Adults. , 2017, Journal of the American Medical Directors Association.

[44]  J. Ansai,et al.  Effects of the Addition of a Dual Task to a Supervised Physical Exercise Program on Older Adults' Cognitive Performance. , 2017, Journal of aging and physical activity.

[45]  P. Haggard Sense of agency in the human brain , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  Mirta Fiorio,et al.  Individual Differences in the Rubber Hand Illusion Are Related to Sensory Suggestibility , 2016, PloS one.

[47]  Mel Slater,et al.  Virtual Embodiment of White People in a Black Virtual Body Leads to a Sustained Reduction in Their Implicit Racial Bias , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[48]  Kazuki Hyodo,et al.  Possible neurophysiological mechanisms for mild-exercise-enhanced executive function: An fNIRS neuroimaging study , 2016 .

[49]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Addition of a non-immersive virtual reality component to treadmill training to reduce fall risk in older adults (V-TIME): a randomised controlled trial , 2016, The Lancet.

[50]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  The sense of body ownership relaxes temporal constraints for multisensory integration , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[51]  Mel Slater,et al.  First Person Perspective of Seated Participants Over a Walking Virtual Body Leads to Illusory Agency Over the Walking , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[52]  A. Berti,et al.  Sensing the body, representing the body: Evidence from a neurologically based delusion of body ownership , 2016, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[53]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Embodying Others in Immersive Virtual Reality: Electro-Cortical Signatures of Monitoring the Errors in the Actions of an Avatar Seen from a First-Person Perspective , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  B. Hommel,et al.  The role of agency for perceived ownership in the virtual hand illusion , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[55]  A. Berti,et al.  A predictive nature for tactile awareness? Insights from damaged and intact central-nervous-system functioning , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[56]  F. Garbarini,et al.  Are Movements Necessary for the Sense of Body Ownership? Evidence from the Rubber Hand Illusion in Pure Hemiplegic Patients , 2015, PLoS ONE.

[57]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Changing bodies changes minds: owning another body affects social cognition , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  Mel Slater,et al.  Body ownership causes illusory self-attribution of speaking and influences subsequent real speaking , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Resolved but not forgotten: Stroop conflict dredges up the past , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[60]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: An fNIRS study , 2014, NeuroImage.

[61]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: Comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[62]  O. Blanke,et al.  Illusory self-identification with an avatar reduces arousal responses to painful stimuli , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[63]  Yosuke Sakairi,et al.  Development of the Two‐Dimensional Mood Scale for self‐monitoring and self‐regulation of momentary mood states , 2013 .

[64]  Tabitha C. Peck,et al.  Putting yourself in the skin of a black avatar reduces implicit racial bias , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[65]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  What Color is My Arm? Changes in Skin Color of an Embodied Virtual Arm Modulates Pain Threshold , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[66]  M. Slater,et al.  Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[67]  M. Slater,et al.  The building blocks of the full body ownership illusion , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[68]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  The person in the mirror: Using the enfacement illusion to investigate the experiential structure of self-identification , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[69]  I. Dan,et al.  Acute moderate exercise enhances compensatory brain activation in older adults , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[70]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Extending Body Space in Immersive Virtual Reality: A Very Long Arm Illusion , 2012, PloS one.

[71]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Moving a Rubber Hand that Feels Like Your Own: A Dissociation of Ownership and Agency , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[72]  C. Spence,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2022 .

[73]  A. Arnsten Catecholamine Influences on Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortical Networks , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[74]  Arne Dietrich,et al.  The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[75]  P. Haggard,et al.  Having a body versus moving your body: Neural signatures of agency and body-ownership , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[76]  P. Tomporowski,et al.  The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis , 2010, Brain Research.

[77]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test , 2010, NeuroImage.

[78]  M. Tsakiris My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of body-ownership , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[79]  F. Vignemont Body schema and body image—Pros and cons , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[80]  Mel Slater,et al.  Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[81]  C. Moore,et al.  Movement and the Rubber Hand Illusion , 2009, Perception.

[82]  P. Haggard,et al.  Sense of Agency Primes Manual Motor Responses , 2009, Perception.

[83]  C. Spence,et al.  Psychologically induced cooling of a specific body part caused by the illusory ownership of an artificial counterpart , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[84]  H. Ehrsson The Experimental Induction of Out-of-Body Experiences , 2007, Science.

[85]  C. Spence,et al.  Vision and touch in ageing: Crossmodal selective attention and visuotactile spatial interactions , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[86]  P. Haggard,et al.  Having a body versus moving your body: How agency structures body-ownership , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[87]  M. Seghier,et al.  Illusory persistence of touch after right parietal damage: neural correlates of tactile awareness. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[88]  A. Miyake,et al.  The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[89]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[90]  Frithjof Kruggel,et al.  Near‐infrared spectroscopy can detect brain activity during a color–word matching Stroop task in an event‐related design , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[91]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[92]  M. Tamura,et al.  Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[93]  G. Lohmann,et al.  Color-Word Matching Stroop Task: Separating Interference and Response Conflict , 2001, NeuroImage.

[94]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[95]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[96]  S. Gallagher Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[97]  D. Wegner,et al.  Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[98]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[99]  G. Breithardt,et al.  Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. , 1996 .

[100]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Availability of visual and proprioceptive afferent messages and postural control in elderly adults , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[101]  E. Watanabe,et al.  Spatial and temporal analysis of human motor activity using noninvasive NIR topography. , 1995, Medical physics.

[102]  S. Shoemaker The First-Person Perspective , 1994 .

[103]  S. Arridge,et al.  Estimation of optical pathlength through tissue from direct time of flight measurement. , 1988, Physics in medicine and biology.

[104]  I. Rock,et al.  Vision and touch. , 1967, Scientific American.

[105]  Raymond B. Cattell,et al.  Psychological measurement: normative, ipsative, interactive. , 1944 .

[106]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  The projected hand illusion: component structure in a community sample and association with demographics, cognition, and psychotic-like experiences , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[107]  R. Harper A Sense of Body , 2011 .

[108]  Thomas M Seed,et al.  Acute Effects , 2011 .

[109]  F. de Vignemont,et al.  Body schema and body image--pros and cons. , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[110]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[111]  H. Jasper Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography , 1958 .