Stop or stop-change--does it make any difference for the inhibition process?

In the course of daily living altering environmental conditions or changing internal states often cause us to abandon obsolete planned or initiated actions and force us to update our behavior. The stop-change paradigm, an extension of the very popular stop-signal paradigm, allows the investigation of response inhibition and response re-engagement in a laboratory setting. This is very important as everyday life rarely calls for the complete suppression of actions without subsequent behavioral adjustment. The present review first gives a short overview on stop-signal inhibition and its behavioral and neural models before summarizing the findings in the stop-change domain. In doing so, this review shall contribute to a better understanding of the functional and neural architecture of response inhibition and re-engagement. Moreover, the question is raised whether the same or different inhibition process(es) are involved in stop-signal- and stop-change inhibition.

[1]  B. Forstmann,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: resisting the call of the Sirens. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[2]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .

[4]  C. Li,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Functional Connectivity Delineates Distinct Roles of the Inferior Frontal Cortex and Presupplementary Motor Area in Stop Signal Inhibition , 2022 .

[5]  H. Engeland,et al.  Dose-related effect of methylphenidate on stopping and changing in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2006, European Psychiatry.

[6]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[7]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Effects of methylphenidate on inhibitory control in hyperactive children , 1989, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[8]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions: II. Stop-signal studies of repetition priming. , 1985 .

[9]  Rajita Sinha,et al.  Subcortical processes of motor response inhibition during a stop signal task , 2008, NeuroImage.

[10]  E. Crone,et al.  Hot and Cool Aspects of Cognitive Control in Children with ADHD: Decision-Making and Inhibition , 2006, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[11]  R Tannock,et al.  Methylphenidate and cognitive flexibility: Dissociated dose effects in hyperactive children , 1995, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[12]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[13]  R. Tannock,et al.  Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder , 1995, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[14]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. , 1986 .

[15]  M. Rieger,et al.  Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[16]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Common neural substrates for inhibition of spoken and manual responses. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  H. Engeland,et al.  Effects of methylphenidate, desipramine, and l-dopa on attention and inhibition in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  Matthias L. Schroeter,et al.  Prefrontal brain activation during stop-signal response inhibition: An event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  M. Rieger,et al.  Inhibition of ongoing responses following frontal, nonfrontal, and basal ganglia lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[21]  Daniel S Pine,et al.  Deficits in social cognition and response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[22]  G D Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD + CD, anxious, and control children: a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. , 1998, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[23]  Georgiy Bobashev,et al.  Neurocognitive characterizations of Russian heroin addicts without a significant history of other drug use. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[24]  Jobi S. George,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals dissociable mechanisms for global versus selective corticomotor suppression underlying the stopping of action. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  W. Byblow,et al.  Selective inhibition of movement. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Maria C. van de Laar,et al.  Lifespan Changes in Global and Selective Stopping and Performance Adjustments , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[27]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions: I. Stop-signal studies of decision and memory. , 1983 .

[28]  G. D. Logan,et al.  Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque , 2007, Vision Research.

[29]  R. Tannock,et al.  Test of four hypotheses for the comorbidity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder. , 1995, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[30]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  S. Gauggel,et al.  Lack of Inhibition: A Motivational Deficit in Children With Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Children With Traumatic Brain Injury* , 2000, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[32]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .

[33]  G. Logan,et al.  Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  G. Band,et al.  Inhibitory motor control in stop paradigms: review and reinterpretation of neural mechanisms. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[35]  A. Aron,et al.  Stop the Presses , 2008, Psychological science.

[36]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  How to Stop and Change a Response: the Role of Goal Activation in Multitasking , 2022 .

[37]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Don’t look! don’t touch! inhibitory control of eye and hand movements , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  J L Kenemans,et al.  Stopping and changing in adults with ADHD , 2004, Psychological Medicine.

[39]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  G. Logan,et al.  Converging Evidence for a Fronto-Basal-Ganglia Network for Inhibitory Control of Action and Cognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Trevor W Robbins,et al.  Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[42]  J. Kenemans,et al.  A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor control? , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[43]  Jaap Oosterlaan,et al.  Response inhibition and response re-engagement in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, disruptive, anxious and normal children , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[44]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[45]  Winston D. Byblow,et al.  Primary motor cortex and movement prevention: Where Stop meets Go , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[46]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Herbert Roeyers,et al.  How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[48]  W. Dunlap,et al.  Meta-Analysis of Experiments With Matched Groups or Repeated Measures Designs , 1996 .

[49]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[50]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Preparation to Inhibit a Response Complements Response Inhibition during Performance of a Stop-Signal Task , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Caitlin L. Oldenkamp,et al.  A Proactive Mechanism for Selective Suppression of Response Tendencies , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Aron,et al.  Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[53]  R. Kahn,et al.  Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[54]  G. Logan,et al.  Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[55]  G. Logan,et al.  Development of inhibitory control across the life span. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[56]  Katharine N. Thakkar,et al.  Response Inhibition and Response Monitoring in a Saccadic Countermanding Task in Schizophrenia , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[57]  Adam R Aron,et al.  Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[58]  K. Konrad,et al.  Inhibitory control in children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). , 2000, Brain injury.

[59]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al.  Stimulation of the Subthalamic Region Facilitates the Selection and Inhibition of Motor Responses in Parkinson's Disease , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  A. Aron From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[62]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  G. Gründer,et al.  Serotonergic modulation of response inhibition and re‐engagement? Results of a study in healthy human volunteers , 2010, Human psychopharmacology.

[64]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Effective Connectivity Reveals Important Roles for Both the Hyperdirect (fronto-subthalamic) and the Indirect (fronto-striatal-pallidal) Fronto-basal Ganglia Pathways during Response Inhibition , 2022 .

[65]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit complex movements: A stop-signal study of typewriting. , 1982 .

[66]  Carsten Nicolas Boehler,et al.  Pinning down response inhibition in the brain — Conjunction analyses of the Stop-signal task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[67]  G. Band,et al.  Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.

[68]  G. Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  A. Wagner,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Cognitive Control and Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex: Reflexive Reorienting, Motor Inhibition, and Action Updating , 2022 .

[71]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  Bernd Philippen,et al.  When response inhibition is followed by response reengagement: An event‐related fMRI study , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[73]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[74]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Aging and inhibition: beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. , 1994, Psychology and aging.