Effectiveness of applying 2D static depictions and 3D animations to orthographic views learning in graphical course

This study provides experiment results as an educational reference for instructors to help student obtain a better way to learn orthographic views in graphical course. A visual experiment was held to explore the comprehensive differences between 2D static and 3D animation object features; the goal was to reduce the possible misunderstanding factors in the learning process. This empirical study provided one hundred and twenty Taiwanese freshmen four types of visualization, which includes two 2D static depictions (2DT, 2DR), and two 3D animations (3DT, 3DR), to meet five surface styles on orthographic views. The responses to views ability test and interviews illustrated that applying 3D animations shows better performance in understanding the appearances and features of objects constructed by oblique and double-curved surfaces. The application of 3D animations results also demonstrates a better visual comprehension for students, especially when objects are constructed by the complicated features.

[1]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  The Roles of Mental Animations and External Animations in Understanding Mechanical Systems , 2003 .

[2]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments , 2003 .

[3]  Hans-Peter Seidel,et al.  Visually significant edges , 2010, TAP.

[4]  Samsudin Khairulanuar,et al.  Effects of Training Method and Gender on Learning 2D/3D Geometry , 2010 .

[5]  P. Sutherland,et al.  The application of Piagetian and Neo-Piagetian ideas to further and higher education , 1999 .

[6]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Animation: can it facilitate? , 2002, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[7]  R. Mayer,et al.  Animation as an Aid to Multimedia Learning , 2002 .

[8]  Richard Lowe,et al.  Cueing complex animations: Does direction of attention foster learning processes? , 2011 .

[9]  C J Hamilton,et al.  Beyond sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: the impact of gender trait possession. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[10]  Cecil Howard Jensen,et al.  Engineering Drawing And Design , 1979 .

[11]  F. Paas,et al.  Attention Cueing as a Means to Enhance Learning from an Animation , 2007 .

[12]  Emmanuel Schneider,et al.  Static and Animated Presentations in Learning Dynamic Mechanical Systems. , 2009 .

[13]  J. A. Palyvos,et al.  3D visualization types in multimedia applications for science learning: A case study for 8th grade students in Greece , 2009, Comput. Educ..

[14]  T. Eisenberg,et al.  On spatial visualization in college students. , 1977, The Journal of psychology.

[15]  F. Paas,et al.  Towards a Framework for Attention Cueing in Instructional Animations: Guidelines for Research and Design , 2009 .

[16]  Tzu-Chien Liu,et al.  Split-attention and redundancy effects on mobile learning in physical environments , 2012, Comput. Educ..

[17]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up influences on learning from animations , 2007, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[18]  Frederick Ernest Giesecke,et al.  Engineering Graphics , 1969 .

[19]  Thomas Ewing French,et al.  Engineering Drawing and Graphic Technology , 1972 .

[20]  Richard E. Mayer,et al.  Signaling as a Cognitive Guide in Multimedia Learning , 2001 .

[21]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  An investigation of 2D and 3D spatial and mathematical abilities , 2006 .

[22]  Sheryl A. Sorby,et al.  A Course for the Development of 3-D Spatial Visualization Skills. , 1996 .

[23]  Chun-Yen Chang,et al.  The comparative efficacy of 2D- versus 3D-based media design for influencing spatial visualization skills , 2007, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[24]  Gary R. Morrison,et al.  Principles for using animation in computer-based instruction: theoretical heuristics for effective design , 2002, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[25]  Zainal Abidin Akasah,et al.  Bridging the Spatial Visualisation Skills Gap Through Engineering Drawing using the Whole-to-parts Approach , 2006 .

[26]  Gazi Mahabubul Alam,et al.  Comparative evaluation of the effectiveness of 2 and 3D visualizations in students' understanding of structures of organic molecules , 2010 .

[27]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[28]  Rolf Ploetzner,et al.  Developing and evaluating a strategy for learning from animations , 2010 .

[29]  S. Ainsworth,et al.  Multiple Forms of Dynamic Representation. , 2004 .

[30]  Rolf Plötzner,et al.  A systematic characterisation of expository animations , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[31]  S. Olkun Making connections: improving spatial abilities with engineering drawing activities , 2003 .

[32]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Relative effectiveness of animated and static diagrams: An effect of learner prior knowledge , 2008, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[33]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[34]  Mats Lind,et al.  Evaluating 2D and 3D visualizations of spatiotemporal information , 2010, TAP.

[35]  Thomas Huk,et al.  Who benefits from learning with 3D models? the case of spatial ability , 2006, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..

[36]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Measurement as a Means to Advance Cognitive Load Theory , 2003 .

[37]  Ertu Unver Strategies for the Transition to CAD Based 3D Design Education , 2006 .

[38]  Richard Lowe,et al.  Animation and learning: selective processing of information in dynamic graphics , 2003 .

[39]  Richard Lowe,et al.  Interrogation of a dynamic visualization during learning , 2004 .