Potential explanation of limb combination performance differences for two-limb coordination tasks

Rhythmic two‐limb coordinated movements in the sagittal plane are variable and inaccurate when the movements are in the opposite direction as compared with those in the same direction (directional constraint). The magnitude of directional constraint depends on the particular limb combination. It is prominent in ipsilateral hand‐foot coordination, but minimal in bimanual hand coordination. The reason for such differences remains unclear. In this study, we investigated the possible mechanisms underlying the production of the difference that depend on limb combination. Subjects performed two‐limb rhythmic coordinated movements either in the same or in the opposite direction for three separate limb combinations (bilateral hands, contralateral hand and foot, and ipsilateral hand and foot). For each combination two different tasks were performed. In the first condition, subjects actively moved two limbs (active condition). Second, subjects actively moved one limb in coordination with a passively moved limb (passive condition). In the active condition, the directional constraint was dependent upon the limb combination, as reported in previous studies; the directional constraint was quite prominent in ipsilateral combinations, intermediate in contralateral combinations, and minimal for bilateral combination. However, differences in the directional constraint did not depend on limb combination for any combination in the passive conditions which apparently utilized closed‐loop control. In other word, the difference depending on limb combination disappeared when control strategies become uniformly closed‐loop. Thus, we speculate that the control strategy utilized depends on limb combination in the active condition. Additionally, different mechanisms other than closed‐loop control also would have influence depending on the particular limb combination. This may result in differences in performance depending upon the limb combination.

[1]  J. Kelso,et al.  Phase Transitions and Critical Fluctuations in Rhythmic Coordination of Ipsilateral Hand and Foot. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Age-related differences in inhibitory processes during interlimb coordination , 2009, Brain Research.

[3]  Kento Nakagawa,et al.  Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements , 2013, Physiological reports.

[4]  V. Bigl,et al.  Cholinergic transmission in subcortical and cortical visual centers of rats: No evidence for the involvement of primary optic system , 1977, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  E. Zehr,et al.  Regulation of Arm and Leg Movement during Human Locomotion , 2004, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[6]  Stephan Riek,et al.  Let your feet do the walking: constraints on the stability of bipedal coordination , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  J. Kelso,et al.  Symmetry breaking dynamics of human multilimb coordination. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  S. Swinnen Intermanual coordination: From behavioural principles to neural-network interactions , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Load compensation during homologous and non-homologous coordination , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Tetsuro Muraoka,et al.  Ipsilateral wrist–ankle movements in the sagittal plane encoded in extrinsic reference frame , 2013, Neuroscience Research.

[11]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Differential control of in-phase and anti-phase coupling of rhythmic movements of ipsilateral hand and foot , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Age-related deterioration of coordinated interlimb behavior. , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[13]  W. Byblow,et al.  Phase transitions and postural deviations during bimanual kinesthetic tracking , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  M. Jongmans,et al.  Rhythmic coordination of hand and foot in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. , 2006, Child: care, health and development.

[15]  Paolo Cavallari,et al.  Preferential coupling between voluntary movements of ipsilateral limbs , 1982, Neuroscience Letters.

[16]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination deficits in patients with parkinson's disease during the production of two‐joint oscillations in the sagittal plane , 1997, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[17]  J. Summers,et al.  Effects of attentional prioritisation on the temporal and spatial components of an interlimb circle-drawing task. , 2005, Human movement science.

[18]  Jeffery J. Summers,et al.  Age-related differences in corticospinal excitability and inhibition during coordination of upper and lower limbs , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[19]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Synchrony of hand-foot coupled movements: is it attained by mutual feedback entrainment or by independent linkage of each limb to a common rhythm generator? , 2006, BMC Neuroscience.

[20]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Excitability changes in human corticospinal projections to forearm muscles during voluntary movement of ipsilateral foot , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  The coalition of constraints during coordination of the ipsilateral and heterolateral limbs , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  F. Baldissera,et al.  Postural constraints to coupling of ipsilateral hand–foot movements , 2005, Neuroreport.

[23]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Frequency-induced changes in interlimb interactions: Increasing manifestations of closed-loop control , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Development of temporal and spatial bimanual coordination during childhood. , 2012, Motor control.

[25]  W. Byblow,et al.  Functional connectivity between secondary and primary motor areas underlying hand-foot coordination. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  W. Byblow,et al.  A neurophysiological basis for the coordination between hand and foot movement. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative phase destabilization during interlimb coordination: the disruptive role of kinesthetic afferences induced by passive movement , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  J. Summers,et al.  Attentional costs of coordinating homologous and non-homologous limbs. , 2004, Human movement science.

[29]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Unraveling interlimb interactions underlying bimanual coordination. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.