Representation of Patients’ Hand Modulates Fear Reactions of Patients with Spider Phobia in Virtual Reality

Embodiment (i.e., the involvement of a bodily representation) is thought to be relevant in emotional experiences. Virtual reality (VR) is a capable means of activating phobic fear in patients. The representation of the patient’s body (e.g., the right hand) in VR enhances immersion and increases presence, but its effect on phobic fear is still unknown. We analyzed the influence of the presentation of the participant’s hand in VR on presence and fear responses in 32 women with spider phobia and 32 matched controls. Participants sat in front of a table with an acrylic glass container within reaching distance. During the experiment this setup was concealed by a head-mounted display (HMD). The VR scenario presented via HMD showed the same setup, i.e., a table with an acrylic glass container. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental groups. In one group, fear responses were triggered by fear-relevant visual input in VR (virtual spider in the virtual acrylic glass container), while information about a real but unseen neutral control animal (living snake in the acrylic glass container) was given. The second group received fear-relevant information of the real but unseen situation (living spider in the acrylic glass container), but visual input was kept neutral VR (virtual snake in the virtual acrylic glass container). Participants were instructed to touch the acrylic glass container with their right hand in 20 consecutive trials. Visibility of the hand was varied randomly in a within-subjects design. We found for all participants that visibility of the participant’s hand increased presence independently of the fear trigger. However, in patients, the influence of the virtual hand on fear depended on the fear trigger. When fear was triggered perceptually, i.e., by a virtual spider, the virtual hand increased fear. When fear was triggered by information about a real spider, the virtual hand had no effect on fear. Our results shed light on the significance of different fear triggers (visual, conceptual) in interaction with body representations.

[1]  Mark Jensen,et al.  The Effectiveness of Virtual Reality for Dental Pain Control: A Case Study , 2001, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[2]  P. Lang,et al.  Psychometric description of some specific-fear questionnaires. , 1974 .

[3]  Mathieu Vandenbulcke,et al.  Affective scenes influence fear perception of individual body expressions , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[4]  C. J. McGrath,et al.  Effect of exchange rate return on volatility spill-over across trading regions , 2012 .

[5]  P. Pauli,et al.  Effect of multiple context exposure on renewal in spider phobia. , 2013, Behaviour research and therapy.

[6]  Holger Regenbrecht,et al.  The Experience of Presence: Factor Analytic Insights , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[7]  B. de Gelder,et al.  Social context influences recognition of bodily expressions , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Inducing Illusory Ownership of a Virtual Body , 2009, Front. Neurosci..

[9]  Peter J. Lang,et al.  A Bio‐Informational Theory of Emotional Imagery , 1979 .

[10]  Andreas Mühlberger,et al.  The Impact of Different Perceptual Cues On Fear And Presence In Virtual Reality , 2013, Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine.

[11]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[12]  Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al.  What is embodiment? A psychometric approach , 2008, Cognition.

[13]  O. Van den Bergh,et al.  Reinstatement of fear responses in human aversive conditioning. , 2005, Behaviour research and therapy.

[14]  A. Rizzo,et al.  Affective outcomes of virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety and specific phobias: a meta-analysis. , 2008, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[15]  J Szymanski,et al.  Fear of Spiders Questionnaire. , 1995, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[16]  J. Gore,et al.  Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Katharina Tabbert,et al.  Attention and amygdala activity: an fMRI study with spider pictures in spider phobia , 2009, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[18]  C. Botella,et al.  Virtual reality in the treatment of spider phobia: a controlled study. , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[19]  Andreas Mühlberger,et al.  Temporal dynamics in the relation between presence and fear in virtual reality , 2015, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[20]  J. Wissmann,et al.  Reliabilität und Validität dreier Instrumente zur Messung von Angst vor Spinnen , 2002 .

[21]  G. Bower Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.

[22]  David J. Zielinski,et al.  Spatial proximity amplifies valence in emotional memory and defensive approach-avoidance , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Diana Adler,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 2016 .

[24]  S. Weghorst,et al.  Virtual reality and tactile augmentation in the treatment of spider phobia: a case report. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[25]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  How affective information from faces and scenes interacts in the brain. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[26]  M. Dawson,et al.  The electrodermal system , 2007 .

[27]  Mel Slater,et al.  Measuring Presence: A Response to the Witmer and Singer Presence Questionnaire , 1999, Presence.

[28]  Max M. North,et al.  Brief Virtual Reality Therapy for Public Speaking Anxiety , 2002, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[29]  M. First,et al.  Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders : SCID-I : clinical version : scoresheet , 1997 .

[30]  Sebastian T. Bundschuh,et al.  Reliability and validity of German versions of three instruments measuring fear of spiders , 2002 .

[31]  C. Carver,et al.  Self-focused attention and the experience of emotion: attraction, repulsion, elation, and depression. , 1977, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  G. Alpers,et al.  You See What you Fear: Spiders Gain Preferential Access to Conscious Perception in Spider-Phobic Patients , 2014 .

[33]  A. Hamm,et al.  Startle reflex modulation and autonomic responding during anxious apprehension in panic disorder patients. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Isabelle Viaud-Delmon,et al.  Cynophobic Fear Adaptively Extends Peri-Personal Space , 2014, Front. Psychiatry.

[35]  Matthew Price,et al.  Does engagement with exposure yield better outcomes? Components of presence as a predictor of treatment response for virtual reality exposure therapy for social phobia. , 2011, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[36]  P. Silvia,et al.  Self-Awareness and the Emotional Consequences of Self-Discrepancies By : , 2009 .

[37]  G. Alpers,et al.  The persistence of socially instructed threat: two threat-of-shock studies. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[38]  Henrik M. Peperkorn,et al.  The impact of perception and presence on emotional reactions: a review of research in virtual reality , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[39]  Cristina Botella,et al.  Interfaces That Heal: Coupling Real and Virtual Objects to Treat Spider Phobia , 2003, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[40]  Joseph E LeDoux Rethinking the Emotional Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[41]  P. Wojtkowski Temporal Dynamics , 2017, Encyclopedia of GIS.

[42]  M. Seligman Phobias and preparedness , 1971 .

[43]  P. Haggard,et al.  The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  A. Ohman,et al.  Fear appears fast: temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[45]  Nigel H. Lovell,et al.  Towards photorealistic and immersive virtual-reality environments for simulated prosthetic vision: Integrating recent breakthroughs in consumer hardware and software , 2014, 2014 36th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[46]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  Effects of Stereoscopic Presentation, Image Motion, and Screen Size on Subjective and Objective Corroborative Measures of Presence , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[47]  G. Alpers,et al.  Spiders are special: fear and disgust evoked by pictures of arthropods , 2009 .

[48]  Mel Slater,et al.  A Framework for Immersive Virtual Environments (FIVE): Speculations on the Role of Presence in Virtual Environments , 1997, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[49]  Christopher Caudwell This my hand , 1936 .

[50]  Yvonne de Kort,et al.  Is This My Hand I See Before Me? The Rubber Hand Illusion in Reality, Virtual Reality, and Mixed Reality , 2006, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[51]  R. Dolan,et al.  Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical anxiety response , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[52]  Henrik M. Peperkorn,et al.  Influence of perceptual cues and conceptual information on the activation and reduction of claustrophobic fear. , 2016, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[53]  Joseph E LeDoux Coming to terms with fear , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Y. Coello,et al.  Body Space in Social Interactions: A Comparison of Reaching and Comfort Distance in Immersive Virtual Reality , 2014, PloS one.

[55]  J. Eccles The emotional brain. , 1980, Bulletin et memoires de l'Academie royale de medecine de Belgique.

[56]  B. de Gelder,et al.  Is it the real deal? Perception of virtual characters versus humans: an affective cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[57]  P. Pauli,et al.  Fear reactivation prior to exposure therapy: does it facilitate the effects of VR exposure in a randomized clinical sample? , 2015, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[58]  Heinrich H Bülthoff,et al.  Virtual reality for the psychophysiological assessment of phobic fear: responses during virtual tunnel driving. , 2007, Psychological assessment.

[59]  P. Pauli,et al.  One-session virtual reality exposure treatment for fear of flying: 1-Year follow-up and graduation flight accompaniment effects , 2006 .

[60]  Yun Ling,et al.  Effects of Stereoscopic Viewing on Presence, Anxiety, and Cybersickness in a Virtual Reality Environment for Public Speaking , 2012, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[61]  Andreas Mühlberger,et al.  Triggers of fear: perceptual cues versus conceptual information in spider phobia. , 2014, Journal of clinical psychology.

[62]  Georg W. Alpers,et al.  Defensive activation during the rubber hand illusion: Ownership versus proprioceptive drift , 2015, Biological Psychology.

[63]  G. Alpers,et al.  Emotional pictures and sounds: a review of multimodal interactions of emotion cues in multiple domains , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[64]  Ellen Poliakoff,et al.  The effect of visual threat on spatial attention to touch , 2007, Cognition.

[65]  Self-focused attention and the experience of alcoholic intoxication. , 1988, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[66]  M. Kozak,et al.  Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[67]  Stéphane Bouchard,et al.  Anxiety Increases the Feeling of Presence in Virtual Reality , 2008, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[68]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  The Role of Immersion and Narrative in Mediated Presence: The Virtual Hospital Experience , 2011, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[69]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[70]  David A. Sylvester,et al.  The epidemiology of common fears and phobia. , 1969, Comprehensive psychiatry.