Is There a Social Module? Language, Face Processing, and Theory of Mind in Individuals with Williams Syndrome

Many species can respond to the behavior of their conspecifics. Human children, and perhaps some nonhuman primates, also have the capacity to respond to the mental states of their conspecifics, i.e., they have a theory of mind. On the basis of previous research on the theory-of-mind impairment in people with autism, together with animal models of intentionality, Brothers and Ring (1992) postulated a broad cognitive module whose function is to build representations of other individuals. We evaluate the details of this hypothesis through a series of experiments on language, face processing, and theory of mind carried out with subjects with Williams syndrome, a rare genetic neurodevelopmental disorder resulting in an uneven lin-guisticocognitive profile. The results are discussed in terms of how the comparison of different phenotypes (e.g., Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, autism, and hydrocephaly with associated myelomeningocele) can contribute both to understanding the neuropsychology of social cognition and to current thinking about the purported modularity of the brain.

[1]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[2]  O MARBURG,et al.  The amygdaloid complex. , 1949, Confinia neurologica.

[3]  R. Creery Idiopathic hypercalcaemia of infants. , 1953, Lancet.

[4]  T. Stapleton,et al.  Idiopathic hypercalcaemia in infants. , 1953, Lancet.

[5]  R. Mitchell Idiopathic hypercalcaemia of infants , 1958, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[6]  J. Denie,et al.  Supravalvular Aortic Stenosis , 1958, Circulation.

[7]  J. B. Lowe,et al.  Supravalvular Aortic Stenosis , 1961, Circulation.

[8]  A. Beuren,et al.  THE SYNDROME OF SUPRAVALVULAR AORTIC STENOSIS, PERIPHERAL PULMONARY STENOSIS, MENTAL RETARDATION AND SIMILAR FACIAL APPEARANCE. , 1964, The American journal of cardiology.

[9]  A. Krill Mendelian Inheritance in Man: Catalogs of Autosomal Dominant, Autosomal Recessive and X-Linked Phenotypes , 1967 .

[10]  V. McKusick Mendelian Inheritance in Man: Catalogs of Autosomal Dominant, Autosomal Recessive, and X-Linked Phenotypes , 1983 .

[11]  David W. Smith,et al.  The Williams elfin facies syndrome. A new perspective. , 1975, The Journal of pediatrics.

[12]  J. Garland The New England Journal of Medicine. , 1961, Canadian Medical Association journal.

[13]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[15]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[16]  D. J. Goldstein,et al.  Developmental delays in Williams ("Elfin facies") syndrome. , 1984, Applied research in mental retardation.

[17]  K. Jones,et al.  Imparied calcitonin secretion in patients with Williams syndrome. , 1985, The Journal of pediatrics.

[18]  H. Wimmer,et al.  “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children ☆ , 1985 .

[19]  W. Yule,et al.  THE PSYCHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF INFANTILE HYPERCALCEMIA: A PRELIMINARY INVESTIGATION , 1985 .

[20]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[21]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[22]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1997 .

[23]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[24]  A. Karmiloff-Smith From meta-processes to conscious access: Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data , 1986, Cognition.

[25]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children , 1986 .

[26]  W. Yule,et al.  Cognitive abilities and behavioural characteristics of children with idiopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. , 1987, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[27]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[28]  C A Morris,et al.  Natural history of Williams syndrome: physical characteristics. , 1988, The Journal of pediatrics.

[29]  D. Olson,et al.  Developing theories of mind , 1988 .

[30]  A. Leslie,et al.  Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing , 1988 .

[31]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Dissociation between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndrome , 1988 .

[32]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism. , 1988, The New England journal of medicine.

[33]  R. Mulhern,et al.  COGNITIVE PROCESSING OF CHILDREN WITH WILLIAMS SYNDROME , 1988, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[34]  A. Leslie,et al.  Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. , 1989, Child development.

[35]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Seeing either the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[36]  S. Baron-Cohen The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. , 1989, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[37]  Williams syndrome: Serotonin's association with developmental disabilities , 1989, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[38]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism , 1989 .

[39]  A. L. Leiner,et al.  Reappraising the cerebellum: what does the hindbrain contribute to the forebrain? , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[40]  U. Frith Autism: Explaining the Enigma , 2005 .

[41]  Josef Perner,et al.  Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. , 1989 .

[42]  G. Press,et al.  Methods for measuring brain morphologic features on magnetic resonance images. Validation and normal aging. , 1990, Archives of neurology.

[43]  O. Udwin,et al.  A SURVEY OF ADULTS WITH WILLIAMS SYNDROME AND IDIOPATHIC INFANTILE HYPERCALCAEMIA , 1990, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[44]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. , 2005, American journal of medical genetics. Supplement.

[45]  H. Wellman The Child's Theory of Mind , 1990 .

[46]  J. Diliberti,et al.  Williams syndrome in twins. , 2005, American journal of medical genetics. Supplement.

[47]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations , 1990, Development and Psychopathology.

[48]  U Bellugi,et al.  Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. , 1990, Archives of neurology.

[49]  S. Leekam,et al.  Jokes and lies: Children's understanding of intentional falsehood. , 1991 .

[50]  W. Yule,et al.  A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[51]  F. Happé,et al.  The autobiographical writings of three Asperger syndrome adults : Problems of interpretation and implications for theory , 1991 .

[52]  M. Rutter 15 – Autism as a genetic disorder , 1991 .

[53]  T. L. Jernigan,et al.  Specific neurobehavioral profile of Williams' syndrome is associated with neocerebellar hemispheric preservation , 1992, Neurology.

[54]  P. Green Biology and Cognitive Development: the Case of Face Recognition, Mark H. Johnson, John Morton. Blackwell, Oxford (1991), x, +180. Price £35.00 hardback, £10.95 paperback , 1992 .

[55]  C. Nelson,et al.  Developmental behavioral neuroscience , 1992 .

[56]  L. Brothers,et al.  A Neuroethological Framework for the Representation of Minds , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Terry L. Jernigan,et al.  Language, cognition, and brain organization in a neurodevelopmental disorder. , 1992 .

[58]  Annette Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Can neural selectionism be applied to cognitive development and its disorders , 1992 .

[59]  B. Bower A Child's Theory of Mind , 1993 .

[60]  U Bellugi,et al.  Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and cognitive neuroscience. , 1993, American journal of diseases of children.

[61]  F. Happé Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory , 1993, Cognition.

[62]  Annette Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Within-Domain Dissociations in Williams Syndrome: A Window on the Normal Mind. , 1993 .

[63]  L. Petitto,et al.  On the Ontogenetic Requirements for Early Language Acquisition , 1993 .

[64]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[65]  U Bellugi,et al.  Cerebral morphologic distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. , 1993, Archives of neurology.

[66]  Patricia Spallone,et al.  Hemizygosity at the elastin locus in a developmental disorder, Williams syndrome , 1993, Nature Genetics.

[67]  U Bellugi,et al.  Cytoarchitectonic anomalies in a genetically based disorder: Williams syndrome. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[68]  U Bellugi,et al.  Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[69]  L. Schauble,et al.  Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. , 1994 .

[70]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  A model of the mindreading system: Neuropsychological and neurobiological perspectives. , 1994 .

[71]  Helen J. Neville,et al.  Effects of altered auditory sensitivity and age of language acquisition on the development of language-relevant neural systems: Preliminary studies of Williams syndrome. , 1994 .

[72]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes , 1995 .

[73]  J. Piven Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Autism , 1995 .

[74]  D. Perrett,et al.  Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism , 1997 .