Extrinsic Cues Suppress the Encoding of Intrinsic Cues

Remembering where objects are in space is fundamental to adaptive behavior. Little is known about how intact humans combine information from intrinsic (egocentric) and extrinsic (exocentric, allocentric, or landmark-based) coordinate systems to locate objects. Using a simple location estimation paradigm, this study finds that we mostly remember position in extrinsic coordinates. Intrinsic-coordinate-based mapping of space is less precise in the presence of landmarks or extrinsic cues than in their absence. Thus, not only do extrinsic frames of reference dominate internal representations of space, they suppress intrinsic-based representations as well. We speculate that this dominance-suppression hierarchy undercuts intersystem conflicts and underlies a single, undissociated spatial map in intact humans.

[1]  S. Hecht,et al.  THE INFLUENCE OF LIGHT ADAPTATION ON SUBSEQUENT DARK ADAPTATION OF THE EYE , 1937, The Journal of general physiology.

[2]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension , 1994, Current Biology.

[3]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.

[4]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[5]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Change-blindness as a result of ‘mudsplashes’ , 1999, Nature.

[6]  K. F. Austen,et al.  Oculoparalytic Illusion : Visual-Field Dependent Spatial Mislocalizations by Humans Partially Paralyzed with Curare , 2006 .

[7]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Relative mislocalization of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  J. Crawford,et al.  Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  The effects of landmarks on the performance of delayed and real-time pointing movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  A P Batista,et al.  Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.

[11]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Constrained and unconstrained movements involve different control strategies. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[13]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[15]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[16]  C. R. Fox,et al.  Visually perceived eye level and perceived elevation of objects: Linearly additive influences from visual field pitch and from gravity , 1989, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Schlag,et al.  Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[18]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Neurons in the primate superior colliculus coding for arm movements in gaze-related coordinates. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.

[20]  D H Ballard,et al.  Hand-eye coordination during sequential tasks. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[21]  M. Goodale,et al.  The use of visuomotor cues as a strategy for making perceptual judgments in a patient with visual form agnosia. , 1996 .

[22]  J. T. Enright,et al.  The non-visual impact of eye orientation on eye—hand coordination , 1995, Vision Research.

[23]  Tracy L. Faber,et al.  Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.

[24]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[26]  Jeff B. Pelz,et al.  The role of exocentric reference frames in the perception of visual direction , 1995, Vision Research.

[27]  Timothy L. Hubbard,et al.  Representational momentum and the landmark attraction effect. , 1999 .

[28]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons , 1988, Nature.

[30]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for an eye-centered spherical representation of the visuomotor map. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  P. Viviani,et al.  Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  J F Soechting,et al.  A coordinate system for the synthesis of visual and kinesthetic information , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Spatial invariance of visual receptive fields in parietal cortex neurons , 1997, Nature.

[34]  L F Abbott,et al.  Transfer of coded information from sensory to motor networks , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  J. A. Graziano,et al.  Effect of context and efference copy on visual straight ahead , 1989, Vision Research.

[36]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[37]  D. Kerzel Memory for the position of stationary objects: disentangling foveal bias and memory averaging , 2002, Vision Research.

[38]  Paul Dassonville,et al.  The use of egocentric and exocentric location cues in saccadic programming , 1995, Vision Research.