A new metric for assessing group level participation in fluid teams

Equality of participation is an important factor in the success of multidisciplinary science teams. The typical measure, standard deviation, fails to provide unbiased estimates across groups of different sizes or within groups that change size over time. We propose a new metric of participation equality that takes into account real-world teams that have members come and go naturally over the course of a meeting. This new metric ranges from zero (entirely equal participation) to one (entirely dominated by a single person). This metric is at the group level and for whatever period of time the researcher specifies. Using 11 hours of transcribed utterances from informal, fluid, co-located meetings during the Mars Exploration Rover (MER) mission, we computed this metric for 549 blocks of time. We found that this metric had good convergent validity via having strong positive correlations with both a standard deviation metric of words spoken and participation equality as assessed by two independent coders. It also had good discriminant validity by being uncorrelated with positive and negative affect words, including anxiety and sadness words. Furthermore, when only fluid groups were examined, it maintained a strong correlation with coder-assessed participation. Future research can take advantage of this metric in other settings where team membership is fluid and equality of participation is of interest.

[1]  L. Edna Rogers,et al.  Analysis of Relational Communication in Dyads: New Measurement Procedures , 1975 .

[2]  Wolfgang Stroebe,et al.  How the Group Affects the Mind: A Cognitive Model of Idea Generation in Groups , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[3]  K. Dunbar HOW SCIENTISTS REALLY REASON: SCIENTIFIC REASONING IN REAL-WORLD LABORATORIES , 1995 .

[4]  K. Vohs,et al.  Psychology as the Science of Self-reports and Finger Movements Whatever Happened to Actual Behavior? , 2022 .

[5]  Ute Fischer,et al.  Linguistic correlates of team performance: toward a tool for monitoring team functioning during space missions. , 2007, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[6]  Christian D. Schunn,et al.  A Social-Cognitive Framework of Multidisciplinary Team Innovation , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[7]  G. Stasser,et al.  Expert role assignment and information sampling during collective recall and decision making. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  S. Kiesler,et al.  Group processes in computer-mediated communication☆ , 1986 .

[9]  A. Cropley In Praise of Convergent Thinking , 2006 .

[10]  L. E. Rogers,et al.  A relational approach to interpersonal communication. , 1976 .

[11]  Mark T. Palmer,et al.  Controlling conversations: Turns, topics and interpersonal control , 1989 .

[12]  J. Folger The Effects of Vocal Participation and Questioning Behavior on Perceptions of Dominance. , 1980 .

[13]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Collaborative Discovery in a Scientific Domain , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  D. van Knippenberg,et al.  Work group diversity. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[15]  C. D. De Dreu,et al.  Work group diversity and group performance: an integrative model and research agenda. , 2004, The Journal of applied psychology.

[16]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Psychological aspects of natural language. use: our words, our selves. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  K. Dunbar How scientists think: On-line creativity and conceptual change in science. , 1997 .

[18]  S. Schulz-Hardt,et al.  Group decision making in hidden profile situations: dissent as a facilitator for decision quality. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.