Speech recognition of hearing-impaired listeners: predictions from audibility and the limited role of high-frequency amplification.

Two experiments were conducted to examine the relationship between audibility and speech recognition for individuals with sensorineural hearing losses ranging from mild to profound degrees. Speech scores measured using filtered sentences were compared to predictions based on the Speech Intelligibility Index (SII). The SII greatly overpredicted performance at high sensation levels, and for many listeners, it underpredicted performance at low sensation levels. To improve predictive accuracy, the SII needed to be modified. Scaling the index by a multiplicative proficiency factor was found to be inappropriate, and alternative modifications were explored. The data were best fitted using a method that combined the standard level distortion factor (which accounted for decrease in speech intelligibility at high presentation levels based on measurements of normal-hearing people) with individual frequency-dependent proficiency. This method was evaluated using broadband sentences and nonsense syllables tests. Results indicate that audibility cannot adequately explain speech recognition of many hearing-impaired listeners. Considerable variations from audibility-based predictions remained, especially for people with severe losses listening at high sensation levels. The data suggest that, contrary to the basis of the SII, information contained in each frequency band is not strictly additive. The data also suggest that for people with severe or profound losses at the high frequencies, amplification should only achieve a low or zero sensation level at this region, contrary to the implications of the unmodified SII.

[1]  H. Dillon,et al.  An international comparison of long‐term average speech spectra , 1994 .

[2]  H. Fletcher,et al.  The Perception of Speech and Its Relation to Telephony , 1950 .

[3]  Christine M. Rankovic An Application of the Articulation Index to Hearing Aid Fitting , 1991 .

[4]  G. Studebaker A "rationalized" arcsine transform. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[5]  D D Dirks,et al.  Articulation index predictions of contextually dependent words. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  J W Hall,et al.  Temporal integration, frequency resolution, and off-frequency listening in normal-hearing and cochlear-impaired listeners. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  S Buus,et al.  Temporal integration of trains of tone pulses by normal and by cochlearly impaired listeners. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  C V Pavlovic Use of the articulation index for assessing residual auditory function in listeners with sensorineural hearing impairment. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  C V Pavlovic,et al.  Derivation of primary parameters and procedures for use in speech intelligibility predictions. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  J. C. Steinberg,et al.  Factors Governing the Intelligibility of Speech Sounds , 1945 .

[11]  B C Moore,et al.  Short-term temporal integration: evidence for the influence of peripheral compression. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  R M Cox,et al.  Distribution of short-term rms levels in conversational speech. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  M W Skinner,et al.  Speech intelligibility in noise-induced hearing loss: effects of high-frequency compensation. , 1980, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  D D Dirks,et al.  Recognition of nonsense syllables by hearing-impaired listeners and by noise-masked normal hearers. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  C Ludvigsen Prediction of speech intelligibility for normal-hearing and cochlearly hearing-impaired listeners. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  Harvey Fletcher,et al.  The Perception of Speech Sounds by Deafened Persons , 1952 .

[17]  D Byrne Effects of frequency response characteristics on speech discrimination and perceived intelligibility and pleasantness of speech for hearing-impaired listeners. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  C V Pavlovic,et al.  An articulation index based procedure for predicting the speech recognition performance of hearing-impaired individuals. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  L E Humes,et al.  An evaluation of several rationales for selecting hearing aid gain. , 1986, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[20]  D D Dirks,et al.  Stop-consonant recognition for normal-hearing listeners and listeners with high-frequency hearing loss. II: Articulation index predictions. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  R CARHART,et al.  An improved method for classifying audiograms , 1945, The Laryngoscope.

[22]  P M Zurek,et al.  Consonant reception in noise by listeners with mild and moderate sensorineural hearing impairment. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  J R Dubno,et al.  Comparison of frequency selectivity and consonant recognition among hearing-impaired and masked normal-hearing listeners. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  D D Dirks,et al.  Speech recognition and the Articulation Index for normal and hearing-impaired listeners. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.