The development of problem solving in young children: a critical cognitive skill.

Problem solving is a signature attribute of adult humans, but we need to understand how this develops in children. Tool use is proposed as an ideal way to study problem solving in children less than 3 years of age because overt manual action can reveal how the child plans to achieve a goal. Motor errors are as informative as successful actions. Research is reviewed on intentional actions, beginning with block play and progressing to picking up a spoon in different orientations, and finally retrieving objects with rakes and from inside tubes. Behavioral and kinematic measures of motor action are combined to show different facets of skill acquisition and mastery. We need to design environments that encourage and enhance problem solving from a young age. One goal of this review is to excite interest and spur new research on the beginnings of problem solving and its elaboration during development.

[1]  W. Köhler The Mentality of Apes. , 2018, Nature.

[2]  Wolfgang Köhler,et al.  The mentality of apes, 2nd rev. ed. , 1927 .

[3]  N. Bayley Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development , 1969 .

[4]  J. Bruner,et al.  Learning to use a lever. , 1972, Child development.

[5]  J. Bruner Organization of early skilled action. , 1973, Child development.

[6]  Stephen Michael Kosslyn,et al.  Information representation in visual images , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  Peter K. Smith,et al.  Play and Training in Direct and Innovative Problem Solving. , 1979 .

[8]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy , 1980 .

[9]  C. Hofsten Predictive reaching for moving objects by human infants , 1980 .

[10]  G. Michel 3 – Development of Hand-Use Preference during Infancy , 1983 .

[11]  D. Ashmead,et al.  The development of anticipatory hand orientation during infancy. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Development of visually guided hand orientation in reaching. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[14]  J. Javůrek [Motor development]. , 1988, Ceskoslovenska pediatrie.

[15]  E. Gibson Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. , 1988 .

[16]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Task constraints and infant grip configurations. , 1989, Developmental psychobiology.

[18]  K. Connolly,et al.  The Emergence of a Tool-Using Skill in Infancy. , 1989 .

[19]  Ann L. Brown Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children , 1990 .

[20]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[21]  P. Rochat,et al.  Object representation guides infants' reaching in the dark. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  A. Smitsman,et al.  Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Michael F. Young,et al.  Imagery, action, and young children's spatial orientation: it's not being there that counts, it's what one has in mind. , 1994, Child development.

[24]  P. Rochat,et al.  Multimodal perception in the control of infant reaching. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  B. Hood Gravity rules for 2- to 4-year olds? , 1995 .

[26]  L. Steels,et al.  Grounding adaptive language games in robotic agents , 2006, AAAI 2012.

[27]  G. Caldwell,et al.  From cognition to biomechanics and back: the end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[28]  V. Gullapalli,et al.  Visual Information and Object Size in the Control of Reaching. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  A. Smitsman,et al.  The development of tool use: Changing boundaries between organism and environment. , 1997 .

[30]  Cassandra A. Richards,et al.  The role of imagination in facilitating deductive reasoning in 2-, 3- and 4-year-olds , 1999, Cognition.

[31]  M. E. McCarty,et al.  Problem solving in infancy: the emergence of an action plan. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[32]  J. Lockman A perception--action perspective on tool use development. , 2000, Child development.

[33]  R. Siegler,et al.  ACROSS THE GREAT DIVIDE : BRIDGING THE GAP BETWEEN UNDERSTANDING OF TODDLERS ' AND OLDER CHILDREN ' S THINKING Zhe , 2002 .

[34]  Stephen C. Want,et al.  Learning from other people's mistakes: causal understanding in learning to use a tool. , 2001, Child development.

[35]  M. E. McCarty,et al.  How infants use vision for grasping objects. , 2001, Child development.

[36]  M. E. McCarty,et al.  The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers , 2001 .

[37]  Laura J. Claxton,et al.  Evidence of Motor Planning in Infant Reaching Behavior , 2003, Psychological science.

[38]  Constraints in children's learning to use spoons , 2003 .

[39]  M. E. McCarty,et al.  Reaching beyond spatial perception: Effects of intended future actions on visually guided prehension , 2004 .

[40]  A. Smitsman,et al.  Tool Use and Tool Making: A Developmental Action Perspective , 2005 .

[41]  Rachel Keen,et al.  Facilitating Problem-Solving Performance Among 9- and 12-Month-Old Infants , 2005 .

[42]  David A Rosenbaum,et al.  Grasping movement plans , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[43]  Tool-using in rhesus monkeys and 36-month-old children: Acquisition, comprehension, and individual differences , 2006 .

[44]  D. Corbetta,et al.  Plasticity in the development of handedness: evidence from normal development and early asymmetric brain injury. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[45]  L. Schulz,et al.  Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[46]  Erica L. Wohldmann,et al.  Pushing the limits of imagination: mental practice for learning sequences. , 2007, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.

[47]  D. Singer,et al.  A Mandate for Playful Learning in Preschool: Presenting the Evidence , 2008 .

[48]  Ad W. Smitsman,et al.  Perseveration in Tool Use: A Window for Understanding the Dynamics of the Action-Selection Process , 2008 .

[49]  M. Fischman,et al.  The End-State Comfort Effect in Young Children , 2008, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[50]  Kathleen Florita,et al.  Collapse: How Societies Choose to Fail or Succeed by Jared Diamond (New York: Penguin Group, 2005) , 2009 .

[51]  Laura J. Claxton,et al.  Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[52]  R. Keen,et al.  Movement planning reflects skill level and age changes in toddlers. , 2010, Child development.

[53]  The role of testimony in young children's solution of a gravity-driven invisible displacement task , 2010 .

[54]  R. Keen,et al.  Imagining a way out of the gravity bias: preschoolers can visualize the solution to a spatial problem. , 2011, Child development.

[55]  B. Bertenthal Perception and action. , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[56]  Frank Marchak,et al.  Constraints for Action Selection: Overhand Versus Underhand Grips , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.