Opposing effects of head position on sound localization in blind and sighted human subjects

Up to now, there is an unsolved contradiction between the view that the development of an auditory spatial representation needs calibration by vision and the psychophysical demonstration of quite precise sound localization in early blind humans. The present study provides a link between these two competing conceptions. Two experiments were conducted with congenitally or early blind subjects and sighted controls. In the first experiment, subjects pointed with their head to actual sound sources located in the azimuthal plane. In the second experiment, lateralization of dichotic sound stimuli, presented via headphones, was investigated with variation of head‐to‐trunk position. The results showed opposing systematic errors of sound localization or lateralization, depending on head position, made by blind and sighted subjects. These differences suggest that audiomotor feedback replaces vision so as to calibrate auditory space in blind individuals. That is, in contrast to the widespread opinion of compensation of visual loss by a general sharpening of audition, compensatory plasticity in the blind may specifically be related to enhanced processing of proprioceptive and vestibular information with the auditory spatial input.

[1]  W H Ehrenstein,et al.  Auditory-visual spatial integration: a new psychophysical approach using laser pointing to acoustic targets. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  D. Zambarbieri,et al.  Eye-head coordination toward auditory and visual targets in humans. , 1997, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[3]  R. Töpper,et al.  Role of the Posterior Parietal Cortex in Spatial Hearing , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Sound lateralization during passive whole‐body rotation , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  J. Zwislocki,et al.  Just Noticeable Differences in Dichotic Phase , 1956 .

[6]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Sound localization with eccentric head position , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  W. Yost,et al.  Discrimination of interaural differences of level as a function of frequency. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  G. Cioni,et al.  Role of vision on early motor development: lessons from the blind , 2001, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[10]  R. Held Shifts in binaural localization after prolonged exposures to atypical combinations of stimuli. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.

[11]  W. Ehrenstein,et al.  Influence of head-to-trunk position on sound lateralization , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  A J King,et al.  Improved auditory spatial acuity in visually deprived ferrets , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  M. P. Zwiers,et al.  A Spatial Hearing Deficit in Early-Blind Humans , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Carlile,et al.  Changes induced in the representation of auditory space in the superior colliculus by rearing ferrets with binocular eyelid suture , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  D. Ashmead,et al.  Sound localization and sensitivity to interaural time differences in human infants. , 1991, Child development.

[16]  M. Wertheimer Psychomotor Coordination of Auditory and Visual Space at Birth , 1961, Science.

[17]  K. E. Binns,et al.  The Maturation of the Superior Collicular Map of Auditory Space in the Guinea Pig is Disrupted by Developmental Visual Deprivation , 1990, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Auditory Localization Behaviour in Visually Deprived Cats , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  D. McFadden,et al.  Lateralization of high frequencies based on interaural time differences. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  F L Wightman,et al.  Resolution of front-back ambiguity in spatial hearing by listener and source movement. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  A Positron Emission Tomographic Study of Auditory Localization in the Congenitally Blind , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Stretched and upside-down maps of auditory space in the optic tectum of blind-reared owls; acoustic basis and behavioral correlates , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  Richard Held,et al.  Growth in head size during infancy: Implications for sound localization. , 1988 .

[24]  D W Grantham,et al.  Interaural intensity discrimination: insensitivity at 1000 Hz. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  A. Smit,et al.  Synapse Formation between Central Neurons Requires Postsynaptic Expression of the MEN1 Tumor Suppressor Gene , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Eckart Altenmüller,et al.  Neuroperception: Superior auditory spatial tuning in conductors , 2001, Nature.

[27]  J. Blauert Spatial Hearing: The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization , 1983 .

[28]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Intermodality relations in localization in blind and sighted people , 1970 .

[29]  J Field,et al.  Newborn infants orient to sounds. , 1979, Child development.

[30]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[31]  D R Perrott,et al.  Changes in head position as a measure of auditory localization performance: auditory psychomotor coordination under monaural and binaural listening conditions. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[32]  R. Andersen,et al.  Head position signals used by parietal neurons to encode locations of visual stimuli , 1995, Nature.

[33]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Auditory spatial tuning of cortical neurons is sharpened in cats with early blindness. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  R. G. Klumpp,et al.  Some Measurements of Interaural Time Difference Thresholds , 1956 .

[35]  J. Cruysberg,et al.  Two-dimensional sound-localization behavior of early-blind humans , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  A. Dufour,et al.  Improved auditory spatial sensitivity in near-sighted subjects. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[37]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans , 1999, Nature.