Estimating distance during self-motion: a role for visual-vestibular interactions.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Dora E Angelaki | Gregory C DeAngelis | Paul R MacNeilage | Kalpana Dokka | G. DeAngelis | D. Angelaki | P. MacNeilage | Kalpana Dokka
[1] W. Gogel,et al. EQUIDISTANCE TENDENCY AND ITS CONSEQUENCES. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.
[2] G. DeAngelis,et al. A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] Mark Nawrot,et al. The motion/pursuit law for visual depth perception from motion parallax , 2009, Vision Research.
[4] E. Sture Eriksson. Movement parallax during locomotion , 1974 .
[5] K. Nakayama,et al. Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.
[6] James A. Crowell,et al. Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception , 1999, Vision Research.
[7] Hiroyasu Ujike,et al. Motion Parallax Driven by Head Movements: Conditions for Visual Stability, Perceived Depth, and Perceived Concomitant Motion , 2005, Perception.
[8] W. J. V. Damme,et al. Non-visual Information in Structure-from-motion , 1996, Vision Research.
[9] Herschel W. Leibowitz,et al. Oculomotor adjustments in darkness and the specific distance tendency , 1976 .
[10] Mark Nawrot,et al. Eye movements provide the extra-retinal signal required for the perception of depth from motion parallax , 2003, Vision Research.
[11] Lindsey Joyce,et al. The pursuit theory of motion parallax , 2006, Vision Research.
[12] John M. Foley,et al. Primary Distance Perception , 1978 .
[13] Dale Purves,et al. A statistical explanation of visual space , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[14] G. Johansson,et al. Monocular Movement Parallax and Near-Space Perception , 1973 .
[15] Bernhard J. M. Hess,et al. Self-motion-induced eye movements: effects on visual acuity and navigation , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[16] Fulvio Domini,et al. Matching perceived depth from disparity and from velocity: Modeling and psychophysics. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[17] Dora E Angelaki,et al. Does the Middle Temporal Area Carry Vestibular Signals Related to Self-Motion? , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] J. Droulez,et al. The visual perception of three-dimensional shape from self-motion and object-motion , 1994, Vision Research.
[19] M. Banks,et al. Estimator Reliability and Distance Scaling in Stereoscopic Slant Perception , 1999, Perception.
[20] Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al. A Bayesian model of the disambiguation of gravitoinertial force by visual cues , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al. The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[22] M. Sanders. Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .
[23] Christopher R Fetsch,et al. Dynamic Reweighting of Visual and Vestibular Cues during Self-Motion Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Mark Nawrot,et al. Depth from motion parallax scales with eye movement gain. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[25] Dora E Angelaki,et al. Visual and Nonvisual Contributions to Three-Dimensional Heading Selectivity in the Medial Superior Temporal Area , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] Jack M. Loomis,et al. Absolute motion parallax weakly determines visual scale in real and virtual environments , 1995, Electronic Imaging.
[27] G. DeAngelis,et al. Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] Mark Nawrot,et al. MT Neurons Combine Visual Motion with a Smooth Eye Movement Signal to Code Depth-Sign from Motion Parallax , 2009, Neuron.
[29] Jacques Droulez,et al. Absolute distance perception during in-depth head movement: calibrating optic flow with extra-retinal information , 2002, Vision Research.
[30] H. Ujike,et al. Apparent Depth with Retinal Image Motion of Expansion and Contraction Yoked to Head Movement , 1998, Perception.
[31] H. Ono,et al. Apparent Depth with Motion Aftereffect and Head Movement , 1994, Perception.
[32] M. Landy,et al. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.
[33] Jacques Droulez,et al. Self-motion and the perception of stationary objects , 2001, Nature.
[34] Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al. A neural representation of depth from motion parallax in macaque visual cortex , 2008, Nature.
[35] J. Droulez,et al. Contribution of extraretinal signals to the scaling of object distance during self-motion , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] Walter C. Gogel,et al. A comparison of oculomotor and motion parallax cues of egocentric distance , 1979, Vision Research.
[37] WALTER C. GOGEL,et al. Absolute motion parallax and the specific distal 1 , 1973 .
[38] B Rogers,et al. Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.
[39] R. Weale. Vision. A Computational Investigation Into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. David Marr , 1983 .
[40] Gregory C DeAngelis,et al. Coding of horizontal disparity and velocity by MT neurons in the alert macaque. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[41] G P Bingham,et al. The necessity of a perception-action approach to definite distance perception: monocular distance perception to guide reaching. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] R. Wurtz,et al. The role of disparity-sensitive cortical neurons in signalling the direction of self-motion , 1990, Nature.
[43] J. Koenderink. Optic flow , 1986, Vision Research.
[44] J W Dees. Accuracy of absolute visual distance and size estimation in space as a function of stereopsis and motion parallax. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[45] G. DeAngelis,et al. Fine Discrimination Training Alters the Causal Contribution of Macaque Area MT to Depth Perception , 2008, Neuron.
[46] R. Wurtz,et al. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] Dora E Angelaki,et al. Canal-otolith interactions and detection thresholds of linear and angular components during curved-path self-motion. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[48] R. Hetherington. The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .
[49] Yong Gu,et al. Decoding of MSTd Population Activity Accounts for Variations in the Precision of Heading Perception , 2010, Neuron.
[50] G. DeAngelis,et al. Vestibular Heading Discrimination and Sensitivity to Linear Acceleration in Head and World Coordinates , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[51] G. DeAngelis,et al. Linking Neural Representation to Function in Stereoscopic Depth Perception: Roles of the Middle Temporal Area in Coarse versus Fine Disparity Discrimination , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[52] James M. Hillis,et al. Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[53] S. H. Ferris. Motion parallax and absolute distance. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.
[54] M. Ernst,et al. Focus cues affect perceived depth. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[55] R. Wurtz,et al. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. II. Mechanisms of response selectivity revealed by small-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[56] C. Duffy,et al. Heading representation in MST: sensory interactions and population encoding. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.