Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[2] W. G. Koster,et al. The psychological refractory period , 1966 .
[3] M. C. Smith,et al. The Psychological Refractory Period as a Function of Performance of a First Response , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[4] J. Morton,et al. Categories of interference: verbal mediation and conflict in card sorting. , 1969, British journal of psychology.
[5] Howard E. Egeth,et al. Verbal interference in a perceptual comparison task , 1969 .
[6] W. Chase,et al. Sequential effects in choice reaction time. , 1969 .
[7] Thomas J. Triggs,et al. Capacity sharing and speeded reactions to successive signals. , 1969 .
[8] Paul Bertelson,et al. Refractory period of c-reactions , 1969 .
[9] S. Keele. Attention demands of memory retrieval. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.
[10] E. C. Dalrymple-Alford,et al. The locus of interference in the Stroop and related tasks , 1972 .
[11] D. Meyer,et al. Attention and Performance XIV , 1973 .
[12] Steven W. Keele,et al. Attention and human performance , 1973 .
[13] A. Greenwald,et al. On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.
[14] J. H. Flowers,et al. The use of visual and name codes in scanning and classifying colors , 1976, Memory & cognition.
[15] G. M. Redding,et al. Stroop Effect: Interference and Facilitation with Verbal and Manual Responses , 1977, Perceptual and motor skills.
[16] J. Duncan. Divided attention: the whole is more than the sum of its parts. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] John R. Anderson. Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .
[18] R. B. Freeman,et al. Cognition and Motor Processes , 1983 .
[19] Odmar Neumann,et al. Automatic Processing: A Review of Recent Findings and a Plea for an Old Theory , 1984 .
[20] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[21] C. Eriksen,et al. An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .
[22] John E. Laird,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1986 .
[23] Gordon D. Logan,et al. Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. , 1986 .
[24] D. Navon,et al. Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[26] Lexical context effects in reading and naming : an investigation of the "semantic relatedness paradox" , 1988 .
[27] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[28] J. S. St. James. Observations on the microstructure of response conflict , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[29] W. Prinz,et al. Relationships Between Perception and Action: Current Approaches , 1990 .
[30] W. Prinz. A common-coding approach to perception and action , 1990 .
[31] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[32] James D. St. James. Observations on the microstructure of response conflict. , 1990 .
[33] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[34] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[35] T Hasbroucq,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: toward a conceptual clarification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] H Pashler,et al. Making two responses to a single object: implications for the central attentional bottleneck. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] J. C. Johnston,et al. Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference , 1992 .
[38] R. D. de Jong,et al. Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[40] S Kornblum,et al. The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.
[41] H. Pashler,et al. Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[44] Janeen Baxter,et al. DEPENDENCE AND INDEPENDENCE , 1995 .
[45] Jennifer M. Glass,et al. Adaptive executive control: Flexible multiple-task performance without pervasive immutable response-selection bottlenecks , 1995 .
[46] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. S-R compatibility and response selection , 1995 .
[47] B. Hommel. Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .
[48] W. Prinz,et al. Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .
[49] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Observing one's own action - and what it leads to , 1998 .