The right time and the left time: Spatial associations of temporal cues affect target detection in right brain-damaged patients

Humans map timewords such as “yesterday” or “future” onto a mental timeline that holds temporally earlier events on the left side of space and temporally later events on the right side. The perception of time and spatial mapping both are partially subserved by right temporo-parietal brain regions. We tested stroke patients with right-hemisphere lesions on a spatio-temporal cueing task to see whether spatial associations of noninformative temporal cues would elicit the same cognitive deficits as do typical stimulus-driven exogenous cues. While our right brain-damaged patients were able to maintain a mental timeline with words referring to the past sitting to the left and words referring to the future sitting to the right, we also observed that the typical deficit in disengaging from incongruently cued locations persists for noninformative cues that are mapped onto a mental spatial continuum.

[1]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural Systems for Visual Orienting and Their Relationships to Spatial Working Memory , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Posner,et al.  How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  A Farnè,et al.  Neglect determined by the relative location of responses. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  R A Robb,et al.  Analyze: a comprehensive, operator-interactive software package for multidimensional medical image display and analysis. , 1989, Computerized medical imaging and graphics : the official journal of the Computerized Medical Imaging Society.

[6]  Carson Pun,et al.  Neglected Time: Impaired Temporal Perception of Multisecond Intervals in Unilateral Neglect , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  R. Knight,et al.  Cortical Networks Underlying Mechanisms of Time Perception , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Time: the back-door of perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Jay Pratt,et al.  Time flies like an arrow: Space-time compatibility effects suggest the use of a mental timeline , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  M. Jeannerod Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .

[11]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Involuntary but not voluntary orienting contributes to a disengage deficit in visual neglect , 2010, Cortex.

[12]  P. Nichelli,et al.  Time perception in a neglected space , 1996, Neuroreport.

[13]  L. Gauthier,et al.  The Bells Test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. , 1989 .

[14]  R. Miall,et al.  Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  L. Boroditsky Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors , 2000, Cognition.

[16]  S. Weintraub,et al.  Mental state assessment of young and elderly adults in behavioral neurology , 1985 .

[17]  S. Ferber,et al.  Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.

[18]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Normal and Impaired Reflexive Orienting of Attention after Central Nonpredictive Cues , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Kinsbourne Mechanisms of Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .

[20]  Yvon Keromnes,et al.  Space in Language and Cognition , 2007 .

[21]  S. Levinson,et al.  LANGUAGE AND SPACE , 1996 .

[22]  Jacqueline C Snow,et al.  Goal-driven selective attention in patients with right hemisphere lesions: how intact is the ipsilesional field? , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  K. Booth,et al.  The Neglect Syndrome , 1982, Journal of neurosurgical nursing.

[26]  C. Kennard,et al.  The anatomy of visual neglect. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  What kind of visual spatial attention is impaired in neglect? , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Sylvie Chokron,et al.  Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[30]  L. Boroditsky,et al.  Time in the mind: Using space to think about time , 2008, Cognition.

[31]  Km. Heilmann,et al.  The neglect syndrome , 1985 .

[32]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Comment on "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures" , 2009, Science.

[33]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Alison L. Chasteen,et al.  Thinking of God moves attention , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  M. Posner,et al.  Attentional networks , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.