Associative learning between odorants and mechanosensory punishment in larval Drosophila

SUMMARY We tested whether Drosophila larvae can associate odours with a mechanosensory disturbance as a punishment, using substrate vibration conveyed by a loudspeaker (buzz: ). One odour (A) was presented with the buzz, while another odour (B) was presented without the buzz (A/B training). Then, animals were offered the choice between A and B. After reciprocal training (A/B), a second experimental group was tested in the same way. We found that larvae show conditioned escape from the previously punished odour. We further report an increase of associative performance scores with the number of punishments, and an increase according to the number of training cycles. Within the range tested (between 50 and 200 Hz), however, the pitch of the buzz does not apparently impact associative success. Last, but not least, we characterized odour–buzz memories with regard to the conditions under which they are behaviourally expressed – or not. In accordance with what has previously been found for associative learning between odours and bad taste (such as high concentration salt or quinine), we report that conditioned escape after odour–buzz learning is disabled if escape is not warranted, i.e. if no punishment to escape from is present during testing. Together with the already established paradigms for the association of odour and bad taste, the present assay offers the prospect of analysing how a relatively simple brain orchestrates memory and behaviour with regard to different kinds of ‘bad’ events.

[1]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  A Late-Phase, Long-Term Memory Trace Forms in the γ Neurons of Drosophila Mushroom Bodies after Olfactory Classical Conditioning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Albert Cardona,et al.  Neuronal fiber tracts connecting the brain and ventral nerve cord of the early Drosophila larva , 2009, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[3]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Visual learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[4]  Bertram Gerber,et al.  The Drosophila larva as a model for studying chemosensation and chemosensory learning: a review. , 2007, Chemical senses.

[5]  Gilles Laurent,et al.  painless, a Drosophila Gene Essential for Nociception , 2003, Cell.

[6]  C. Zuker,et al.  Genetic dissection of mechanosensory transduction: Mechanoreception-defective mutations of drosophila , 1994, Neuron.

[7]  Bertram Gerber,et al.  A behavior-based circuit model of how outcome expectations organize learned behavior in larval Drosophila. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[8]  B. Gerber,et al.  Cellular site and molecular mode of synapsin action in associative learning. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[9]  Jérôme Casas,et al.  Substrate vibrations elicit defensive behaviour in leafminer pupae. , 1997, Journal of insect physiology.

[10]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  U. Müller,et al.  Memory , 2006, Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences CMLS.

[12]  E. Buchner,et al.  Behavioral and Synaptic Plasticity Are Impaired upon Lack of the Synaptic Protein SAP47 , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Bertram Gerber,et al.  Olfactory learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[14]  B. Gerber,et al.  Natural variation in Drosophila larval reward learning and memory due to a cGMP-dependent protein kinase. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[15]  M. Bate,et al.  Gateway cloning vectors for the LexA-based binary expression system in Drosophila , 2008, Fly.

[16]  Thomas Preat,et al.  PKA Dynamics in a Drosophila Learning Center: Coincidence Detection by Rutabaga Adenylyl Cyclase and Spatial Regulation by Dunce Phosphodiesterase , 2010, Neuron.

[17]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  Smelling, tasting, learning: Drosophila as a study case. , 2009, Results and problems in cell differentiation.

[18]  Maurice J. Kernan Mechanotransduction and auditory transduction in Drosophila , 2007, Pflügers Archiv - European Journal of Physiology.

[19]  Kara L. Marshall,et al.  The cell biology of touch , 2010, The Journal of cell biology.

[20]  Bertram Gerber,et al.  Outcome expectations drive learned behaviour in larval Drosophila , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  L. Vosshall,et al.  Molecular architecture of smell and taste in Drosophila. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  Kristin Scott Taste Recognition: Food for Thought , 2005, Neuron.

[23]  W. Quinn,et al.  Learning in Normal and Mutant Drosophila Larvae , 1979, Science.

[24]  J Casas,et al.  Matching host reactions to parasitoid wasp vibrations , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  Xiankun Zeng,et al.  Neurexin‐1 is required for synapse formation and larvae associative learning in Drosophila , 2007, FEBS letters.

[26]  Anupama Dahanukar,et al.  Insect odor and taste receptors. , 2006, Annual review of entomology.

[27]  D. Shepherd,et al.  Central afferent projections of proprioceptive sensory neurons in Drosophila revealed with the enhancer‐trap technique , 1996, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  M. Mizunami,et al.  Roles of octopaminergic and dopaminergic neurons in appetitive and aversive memory recall in an insect , 2009, BMC Biology.

[29]  Shawn R. Olsen,et al.  Cracking neural circuits in a tiny brain: new approaches for understanding the neural circuitry of Drosophila , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.

[30]  M. Giurfa Behavioral and neural analysis of associative learning in the honeybee: a taste from the magic well , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[31]  G. Nagel,et al.  Light-Induced Activation of Distinct Modulatory Neurons Triggers Appetitive or Aversive Learning in Drosophila Larvae , 2006, Current Biology.

[32]  R. Menzel Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[33]  S. Waddell,et al.  Drosophila olfactory memory: single genes to complex neural circuits , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  I. Rodriguez,et al.  Pheromone sensing in mice. , 2009, Results and problems in cell differentiation.

[35]  W. Kuebler,et al.  Mechanotransduction by TRP Channels: General Concepts and Specific Role in the Vasculature , 2009, Cell Biochemistry and Biophysics.

[36]  Tomoko Ohyama,et al.  A Combinatorial Semaphorin Code Instructs the Initial Steps of Sensory Circuit Assembly in the Drosophila CNS , 2011, Neuron.

[37]  Feng Zhang,et al.  Nociceptive Neurons Protect Drosophila Larvae from Parasitoid Wasps , 2007, Current Biology.

[38]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  The Role of Dopamine in Drosophila Larval Classical Olfactory Conditioning , 2009, PloS one.

[39]  E. Buchner,et al.  A role for Synapsin in associative learning: the Drosophila larva as a study case. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[40]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  Drosophila Larvae Establish Appetitive Olfactory Memories via Mushroom Body Neurons of Embryonic Origin , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  Electric shock-induced associative olfactory learning in Drosophila larvae. , 2010, Chemical senses.

[42]  H. Tanimoto,et al.  Appetitive and Aversive Visual Learning in Freely Moving Drosophila , 2009, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[43]  Andrew P Jarman,et al.  Studies of mechanosensation using the fly. , 2002, Human molecular genetics.