A Psychophysical Study of Sensory Saltation With an Open Response Paradigm

As we develop new haptic interfaces, it is desirable to present haptic information in an intuitive and effective manner. The sensory saltation phenomenon is a haptic spatiotemporal illusion that, with the appropriate spatial and timing parameters, evoke a powerful perception of directional lines. Efforts are underway to develop a general-purpose haptic display based on sensory saltation that can find application in many areas including a haptic driving navigation guidance system. The current study is designed to test the hypothesis that saltatory signals can be readily perceived by human observers without training. Using a 3-by-3 tactor array, horizontal, vertical and diagonal saltatory lines are generated. An open response paradigm is used to permit subjects to describe saltatory signals with their own imagination. Results show that the saltatory signals used in this study share unique and consistent interpretations among the group of observers tested. Future work include a follow up study of the same saltatory signals using a standard absolute identification paradigm. 1. INTRODUCTION For the past few years, we have been studying the sensory saltation phenomenon in an effort to develop a general-purpose haptic interface that can find a wide range of applications. There are several reasons why we choose to study this phenomenon. Firstly, sensory saltation provides a mechanism for displaying directional information that is highly intuitive. Compared with sensory aids for the deaf (for example, that require a user to learn unfamiliar tactile stimulation patterns, our saltatory display can be readily interpreted by naive observers. Secondly, the sensory saltation illusion can be evoked with relatively simple hardware configurations.

[1]  P J Blamey,et al.  Psychophysical studies relevant to the design of a digital electrotactile speech processor. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  S Weinstein,et al.  Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality , 1968 .

[3]  J. C. Bliss,et al.  A direct translation reading aid for the blind , 1966 .

[4]  G. Brindley “Seeing” with Skin , 1973, Nature.

[5]  Alex Pentland,et al.  Tactual displays for wearable computing , 1997, Digest of Papers. First International Symposium on Wearable Computers.

[6]  F A Geldard,et al.  Saltation in somesthesis. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  Lorraine A. Delhorne,et al.  Current Results of a Field Study of Adult Users of Tactile Aids , 1995 .

[8]  Frank A. Geldard,et al.  The cutaneous saltatory area and its presumed neural basis , 1983 .

[9]  Frank A. Geldard,et al.  Sensory saltation : metastability in the perceptual world , 1977 .

[10]  J. Pickett,et al.  COMMUNICATION OF SPEECH SOUNDS BY A TACTUAL VOCODER. , 1963, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[11]  Alex Pentland,et al.  A wearable haptic navigation guidance system , 1998, Digest of Papers. Second International Symposium on Wearable Computers (Cat. No.98EX215).

[12]  R W Cholewiak,et al.  The generation of vibrotactile patterns on a linear array: Influences of body site, time, and presentation mode , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  Thomas H. Massie,et al.  The PHANToM Haptic Interface: A Device for Probing Virtual Objects , 1994 .

[14]  F A Geldard,et al.  The mutability of time and space on the skin. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  F. A. Geldard,et al.  The Cutaneous "Rabbit": A Perceptual Illusion , 1972, Science.

[16]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .