Cross-Lagged Relation of Leisure Activity Participation to Trail Making Test Performance 6 Years Later: Differential Patterns in Old Age and Very Old Age

Objective: We investigated cross-lagged relations between leisure activity participation and Trail Making Test (TMT) performance over 6 years and whether those reciprocal associations differed between individuals. Method: We analyzed data from 232 participants tested on performance in TMT Parts A and B as well as interviewed on leisure activity participation in 2 waves 6 years apart. Mean age in the Wave 1 was 73.42 years. Participants were also tested on vocabulary (Mill Hill scale) as a proxy indicator of crystallized intelligence and reported information on early and midlife cognitive reserve markers (education and occupation). Latent cross-lagged models were applied to investigate potential reciprocal activity−TMT relationships. Results: The relation of leisure activity participation predicting TMT performance 6 years later was significantly larger than was the relation of TMT performance predicting later leisure activity participation. Statistically comparing different moderator groups revealed that this pattern was evident both in individuals with low education and in those with high education but, notably, emerged in only young-old adults (but not in old-old adults), in individuals with a low cognitive level of job in midlife (but not in those with a high cognitive level of job in midlife), and in individuals with high scores in vocabulary (but not in those with low scores in vocabulary). Conclusions: Late-life leisure activity participation may predict later cognitive status in terms of TMT performance, but individuals may markedly differ with respect to such effects. Implications for current cognitive reserve and neuropsychological aging research are discussed.

[1]  M. Kliegel,et al.  Development of reserves over the life course and onset of vulnerability in later life , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.

[2]  E. Bodner,et al.  The reciprocal relationship between participation in leisure activities and cognitive functioning: the moderating effect of self-rated literacy level , 2017, Aging & mental health.

[3]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  An approach to studying the neural correlates of reserve , 2017, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[4]  B. Johansson,et al.  IQ as Moderator of Terminal Decline in Perceptual and Motor Speed, Spatial, and Verbal Ability: Testing the Cognitive Reserve Hypothesis in a Population-Based Sample Followed From Age 70 Until Death , 2017, Psychology and aging.

[5]  Stuart J. Ritchie,et al.  Do Cognitive and Physical Functions Age in Concert from Age 70 to 76? Evidence from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 , 2016, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[6]  M. Kliegel,et al.  Investigating Discontinuity of Age Relations in Cognitive Functioning, General Health Status, Activity Participation, and Life Satisfaction between Young-Old and Old-Old Age , 2016, International journal of environmental research and public health.

[7]  M. Kliegel,et al.  The association of timing of retirement with cognitive performance in old age: the role of leisure activities after retirement , 2016, International Psychogeriatrics.

[8]  L. Fratiglioni,et al.  Three-year changes in leisure activities are associated with concurrent changes in white matter microstructure and perceptual speed in individuals aged 80 years and older , 2016, Neurobiology of Aging.

[9]  C. Hertzog,et al.  The Importance of Leisure Activities in the Relationship between Physical Health and Well-Being in a Life Span Sample , 2016, Gerontology.

[10]  L. Clare,et al.  Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: a meta-analysis , 2016, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[11]  M. Kliegel,et al.  The Association of Leisure Activities in Middle Adulthood with Cognitive Performance in Old Age: The Moderating Role of Educational Level , 2015, Gerontology.

[12]  Ellen L Hamaker,et al.  A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. , 2015, Psychological methods.

[13]  Mark A Espeland,et al.  Concurrent and longitudinal relationships between cognitive activity, cognitive performance, and brain volume in older adult women. , 2014, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[14]  M. Oris,et al.  "Vivre/Leben/Vivere": An interdisciplinary survey addressing progress and inequalities of aging over the past 30 years in Switzerland. , 2014, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[15]  K. Anstey,et al.  Preserved differentiation between physical activity and cognitive performance across young, middle, and older adulthood over 8 years. , 2014, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[16]  Rudolf Winter-Ebmer,et al.  Does Education Improve Cognitive Performance Four Decades After School Completion? , 2014, Demography.

[17]  David A. Bennett,et al.  Life-span cognitive activity, neuropathologic burden, and cognitive aging , 2013, Neurology.

[18]  D. Bennett,et al.  Life-span cognitive activity, neuropathologic burden and cognitive aging , 2013, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[19]  S. Perelman,et al.  Occupational activity and cognitive reserve: implications in terms of prevention of cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease , 2013, Clinical interventions in aging.

[20]  Hui-Xin Wang,et al.  Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. , 2013, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[21]  M. Ferry,et al.  Mediterranean diet and cognitive function: a French study. , 2013, American Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[22]  Y. Stern Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease , 2012, The Lancet Neurology.

[23]  F. Constantinidou,et al.  The Effects of Age and Education on Executive Functioning and Oral Naming Performance in Greek Cypriot Adults: The Neurocognitive Study for the Aging , 2012, Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica.

[24]  Hui-Xin Wang An active lifestyle postpones dementia onset by more than one year in very old adults , 2012, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[25]  A. Hofman,et al.  Concurrent and longitudinal bidirectional relationships between toddlers' chronic pain and mental health: the generation R study. , 2012, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[26]  Yves Rosseel,et al.  lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling , 2012 .

[27]  H. Christensen,et al.  Activity engagement is related to level, but not change in cognitive ability across adulthood. , 2012, Psychology and aging.

[28]  Alia Ahmed,et al.  Brain Structure and Function in MPS II , 2012 .

[29]  P. Thomas,et al.  Gender, social engagement, and limitations in late life. , 2011, Social science & medicine.

[30]  A. Soubelet Engaging in cultural activities compensates for educational differences in cognitive abilities , 2011, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[31]  David Bartrés-Faz,et al.  Structural and Functional Imaging Correlates of Cognitive and Brain Reserve Hypotheses in Healthy and Pathological Aging , 2011, Brain Topography.

[32]  Indira Fako,et al.  Relationship between Cattell’s 16PF and fluid and crystallized intelligence , 2011 .

[33]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Developmental change and intraindividual variability: relating cognitive aging to cognitive plasticity, cardiovascular lability, and emotional diversity. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[34]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Terminal pathologies affect rates of decline to different extents and age accelerates the effects of terminal pathology on cognitive decline. , 2011, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[35]  C. Sorg,et al.  [Cognitive reserve and its relevance for the prevention and diagnosis of dementia]. , 2011, Der Nervenarzt.

[36]  Christopher Hertzog,et al.  Assessing adult leisure activities: an extension of a self-report activity questionnaire. , 2010, Psychological assessment.

[37]  M. Lindeboom,et al.  The Effect of Retirement on Cognitive Functioning , 2009, Health economics.

[38]  J. Molinuevo,et al.  Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease , 2009, Neurobiology of Aging.

[39]  M. Kliegel,et al.  Age effects in prospective memory performance within older adults: the paradoxical impact of implementation intentions , 2009, European journal of ageing.

[40]  C. Chicherio,et al.  Développement intellectuel au cours du cycle de vie : sources de variabilité et niveaux d'analyse , 2008 .

[41]  A. Kramer,et al.  Enrichment Effects on Adult Cognitive Development , 2008, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[42]  Megan C. Janke,et al.  Modeling Change in Older Adults' Leisure Activities , 2006 .

[43]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry , 2006, Psychological Medicine.

[44]  Laura Fratiglioni,et al.  Mental, Physical and Social Components in Leisure Activities Equally Contribute to Decrease Dementia Risk , 2006, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[45]  E. Kemps,et al.  General lifestyle activities as a predictor of current cognition and cognitive change in older adults: a cross-sectional and longitudinal examination. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[46]  T. Tombaugh Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. , 2004, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[47]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Static and Dynamic Longitudinal Structural Analyses of Cognitive Changes in Old Age , 2003, Gerontology.

[48]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  New Frontiers in the Future of Aging: From Successful Aging of the Young Old to the Dilemmas of the Fourth Age , 2003, Gerontology.

[49]  D. Deeg,et al.  Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[50]  Y. Stern What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[51]  S. Jay Olshansky,et al.  Prospects for Human Longevity , 2001, Science.

[52]  Wolzt,et al.  World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. , 2003, The Journal of the American College of Dentists.

[53]  R. Dixon,et al.  Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[54]  Margret M. Baltes,et al.  The psychology of the oldest-old: the fourth age , 1998 .

[55]  A. Yashin,et al.  Biodemographic trajectories of longevity. , 1998, Science.

[56]  E. Capitani,et al.  Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls , 1996, The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences.

[57]  M Orrell,et al.  Education and dementia , 1995, BMJ.

[58]  M Orrell,et al.  EDUCATION AND DEMENTIA - RESEARCH EVIDENCE SUPPORTS THE CONCEPT USE IT OR LOSE IT , 1995 .

[59]  A. Mackinnon,et al.  Age differences and interindividual variation in cognition in community-dwelling elderly. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[60]  D. Rogosa A critique of cross-lagged correlation , 1980 .

[61]  R. Reitan Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage , 1958 .

[62]  P. Mecocci,et al.  Short-Term Response is not Predictive of Long-Term Response to Acetylcholinesterase Inhibitors in Old Age Subjects with Alzheimer's Disease: A "Real World" Study. , 2017, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[63]  M. Studer,et al.  The persistence of social stratification? A life course perspective on old-age poverty in Switzerland , 2015 .

[64]  Patricia A Tun,et al.  Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. , 2010, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[65]  M. Lerch,et al.  La transition ultime. Longévité et mortalité aux grands âges dans le bassin lémanique , 2009 .

[66]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Cognitive Reserve: Implications for Assessment and Intervention , 2013, Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica.

[67]  P. Bentler,et al.  Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives , 1999 .

[68]  P. Keyes,et al.  Critique of the , 1981 .