Practice makes transfer of motor skills imperfect

We investigated the practice-effects on motor skill transfer and the associated representational memory changes that occur during the within-practice and between-practice phases. In two experiments, participants produced extension–flexion movements with their dominant right arm for a limited or prolonged practice session arranged in either a single- or multi-session format. We tested the ability of participants to transfer the original pattern (extrinsic transformation) or the mirrored one (intrinsic transformation) to the non-dominant left arm, 10 min and 24 h after the practice sessions. Results showed that practice induces rapid motor skill improvements that are non-transferable irrespective of the amount of acquisition trials. Furthermore, the extrinsic component of the skill develops early and remains the dominant coding system during practice. Conversely, we found distinct between-practice memory changes: a limited practice induces an off-line development of the extrinsic component, whereas a prolonged practice session subserves the off-line development of the intrinsic component (Experiment 2). We provided further evidence that the long-term representation of the motor skill also depends on the nature of the practice session itself: the parsing of practice into multiple sessions narrows the effector-transfer capacities in comparison to a single session (Experiment 1). These findings yield theoretical and practical implications that are discussed in the context of recent motor skill learning models.

[1]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Consolidation of motor memory , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[2]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Limitations in interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Stefan Panzer,et al.  Role of action observation and action in sequence learning and coding. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Henry L. Roediger,et al.  Generalizing test-enhanced learning from the laboratory to the classroom , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[6]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Awareness Modifies the Skill-Learning Benefits of Sleep , 2004, Current Biology.

[7]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  When Learning and Remembering Compete: A Functional MRI Study , 2009, PLoS biology.

[8]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Handedness: Differential Specializations for Control of Trajectory and Position , 2005, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[9]  Jeffrey D. Karpicke,et al.  The Power of Testing Memory Basic Research and Implications for Educational Practice , 2006 .

[10]  Alexander M. Harner,et al.  Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  L. Cohen,et al.  Modification of Existing Human Motor Memories Is Enabled by Primary Cortical Processing during Memory Reactivation , 2010, Current Biology.

[12]  E. Robertson From Creation to Consolidation: A Novel Framework for Memory Processing , 2009, PLoS biology.

[13]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Motor Performance and Motor Learning in the Primary Motor Cortex of Monkeys Adapting to an External Force Field , 2001, Neuron.

[14]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[15]  Tamar Flash,et al.  Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  G. Deuschl,et al.  Sleep consolidates the effector-independent representation of a motor skill , 2010, Neuroscience.

[17]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  J. Donoghue,et al.  Plasticity and primary motor cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  Jason B. Boyle,et al.  Coding of on-line and pre-planned movement sequences. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[20]  Charles H Shea,et al.  The coding and effector transfer of movement sequences. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  S. Keele,et al.  The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. , 2003, Psychological review.

[23]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. , 2007, Sleep medicine.

[24]  M. Weigelt,et al.  Acquisition of a Complex Basketball-Dribbling Task in School Children as a Function of Bilateral Practice Order , 2011, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[25]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[26]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation , 2003, Nature.

[27]  A. C. Butler,et al.  The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[29]  Susanne Diekelmann,et al.  Slow-wave sleep takes the leading role in memory reorganization , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Current concepts in procedural consolidation , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  Jeffrey D. Karpicke,et al.  Test-Enhanced Learning , 2006, Psychological science.

[32]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[33]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  M. Ewers,et al.  Different rates of forgetting following study versus test trials , 2003, Memory.

[36]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Representation of movement sequences is related to task characteristics. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[38]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[39]  X. Bosch,et al.  Imaging the brain. , 1998, The New England journal of medicine.

[40]  Yadin Dudai,et al.  Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[41]  J. Przybysławski,et al.  Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[42]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Off-Line Learning and the Primary Motor Cortex , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  Shana K. Carpenter,et al.  Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: the benefits of elaborative retrieval. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Altering memory representations through retrieval. , 1985 .

[46]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Distinct consolidation outcomes in a visuomotor adaptation task: Off-line leaning and persistent after-effect , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[47]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Long-term retention of motor skill in macaque monkeys and humans , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  Stefan Panzer,et al.  Inter-manual transfer and practice: coding of simple motor sequences. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[49]  David A. Caulton,et al.  On the Modularity of Sequence Representation , 1995 .

[50]  S. Sara Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. , 2000, Learning & memory.

[51]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Imaging Brain Plasticity during Motor Skill Learning , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[52]  M. Desseilles,et al.  Both the Hippocampus and Striatum Are Involved in Consolidation of Motor Sequence Memory , 2008, Neuron.

[53]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations depends on handedness , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  J. D. McGaugh Memory--a century of consolidation. , 2000, Science.

[55]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep-Dependent Learning and Memory Consolidation , 2004, Neuron.

[56]  Y. Dudai,et al.  Rites of Passage of the Engram Reconsolidation and the Lingering Consolidation Hypothesis , 2004, Neuron.

[57]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning , 1995, Nature.

[58]  J. Born,et al.  The memory function of sleep , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[60]  Christoph Braun,et al.  EEG correlates of coordinate processing during intermanual transfer , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[62]  Andrew C Butler,et al.  Repeated testing produces superior transfer of learning relative to repeated studying. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[63]  V. Penhune,et al.  Specific Increases within Global Decreases: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Five Days of Motor Sequence Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Doug Rohrer,et al.  Tests enhance the transfer of learning. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.