Incidental regulation of attraction : The neural basis of the derogation of attractive alternatives in romantic relationships

Although a great deal of research addresses the neural basis of deliberate and intentional emotion-regulation strategies, less attention has been paid to the neural mechanisms involved in implicit forms of emotion regulation. Behavioural research suggests that romantically involved participants implicitly derogate the attractiveness of alternative partners, and the present study sought to examine the neural basis of this effect. Romantically committed participants in the present study were scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while indicating whether they would consider each of a series of attractive (or unattractive) opposite-sex others as a hypothetical dating partner both while under cognitive load and no cognitive load. Successful derogation of attractive others during the no cognitive load compared to the cognitive load trials corresponded with increased activation in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and posterior dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (pDMPFC), and decreased activation in the ventral striatum, a pattern similar to those reported in deliberate emotion-regulation studies. Activation in the VLPFC and pDMPFC was not significant in the cognitive load condition, indicating that while the derogation effect may be implicit, it nonetheless requires cognitive resources. Additionally, activation in the right VLPFC correlated with participants' level of relationship investment. These findings suggest that the RVLPFC may play a particularly important role in implicitly regulating the emotions that threaten the stability of a romantic relationship.

[1]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  Ross A. Thompson,et al.  Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations , 2007 .

[3]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Harmon-Jones, Harmon-Jones / COGNITIVE DISSONANCE MODEL Testing the Action-Based Model of Cognitive Dissonance: The Effect of Action Orientation on Postdecisional Attitudes , 2002 .

[4]  Suzanne N Haber,et al.  Neurocircuitry: A Window into the Networks Underlying Neuropsychiatric Disease , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[5]  S. Koole The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review , 2009 .

[6]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Putting Feelings Into Words , 2007, Psychological science.

[7]  Johan C. Karremans,et al.  The role of self-regulation in derogating attractive alternatives. , 2010 .

[8]  J. Gross Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  E. Kandel,et al.  Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activity in the Amygdala , 2006, Neuron.

[10]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[11]  R. Baumeister,et al.  High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[12]  J. G. Carlson,et al.  Psychology of Emotion , 1991 .

[13]  C. Sibley,et al.  Attachment and attraction toward romantic partners versus relevant alternatives within daily interactions , 2008 .

[14]  J. Gross The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review , 1998 .

[15]  Bharat Maldé,et al.  Interpersonal relations — a theory of interdependence , 1979 .

[16]  H. Kelley,et al.  Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence , 1978 .

[17]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  The Neural Bases of Distraction and Reappraisal , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  S. Koole,et al.  Getting a grip on your feelings: effects of action orientation and external demands on intuitive affect regulation. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processes in Emotion Generation: Common and Distinct Neural Mechanisms , 2009 .

[20]  Kevin Hammond,et al.  Getting a grip , 1997, Science.

[21]  D. Sepinwall,et al.  The Commitment Calibration Hypothesis: When Do People Devalue Attractive Alternatives? , 1999 .

[22]  Thijs Verwijmeren,et al.  Mimicking Attractive Opposite-Sex Others: The Role of Romantic Relationship Status , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[23]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[24]  Andrew L. Alexander,et al.  Contextual Modulation of Amygdala Responsivity to Surprised Faces , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Frank H Wilhelm,et al.  How to Bite Your Tongue Without Blowing Your Top: Implicit Evaluation of Emotion Regulation Predicts Affective Responding to Anger Provocation , 2006, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[26]  Jody L. Davis,et al.  Commitment and relationship maintenance mechanisms , 2001 .

[27]  K. Fernow New York , 1896, American Potato Journal.

[28]  K. Lambert Handbook of Emotion Regulation , 2007 .

[29]  Geertjan Overbeek,et al.  What it takes to forgive: When and why executive functioning facilitates forgiveness. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  J. Brehm Postdecision changes in the desirability of alternatives. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[31]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Strategic Automation of Emotion Regulation Approaches to Emotion Regulation , 2022 .

[32]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  J. Gross Emotion Regulation: Past, Present, Future , 1999 .

[34]  S. Haber,et al.  The Reward Circuit: Linking Primate Anatomy and Human Imaging , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[35]  Andreas Voss,et al.  Counter-regulation in affective attentional biases: a basic mechanism that warrants flexibility in emotion and motivation. , 2008, Emotion.

[36]  Andreas B. Eder,et al.  Automatic influence of arousal information on evaluative processing: Valence–arousal interactions in an affective Simon task , 2010 .

[37]  R. Dolan,et al.  Psychology: Reward value of attractiveness and gaze , 2001, Nature.

[38]  K. Strongman,et al.  The psychology of emotion , 1973 .

[39]  A. Ward,et al.  Don't mind if I do: disinhibited eating under cognitive load. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[41]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Tools of the Trade Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research : rebalancing the scale , 2009 .

[42]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  Neural Systems Supporting the Control of Affective and Cognitive Conflicts , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  M. L. V. Leeuwen,et al.  IS BEAUTIFUL ALWAYS GOOD? IMPLICIT BENEFITS OF FACIAL ATTRACTIVENESS , 2004 .

[44]  C. Rusbult,et al.  Resisting temptation: Devaluation of alternative partners as a means of maintaining commitment in close relationships. , 1989, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

[45]  Simon van Gaal,et al.  Unconscious Activation of the Prefrontal No-Go Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Bess Williamson,et al.  Getting a Grip , 2012, Winterthur Portfolio.

[47]  T. Egner,et al.  Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[48]  J. Gross,et al.  Automatic emotion regulation , 2005 .

[49]  Elliot T. Berkman,et al.  Inhibitory spillover: Intentional motor inhibition produces incidental limbic inhibition via right inferior frontal cortex , 2009, NeuroImage.

[50]  Gráinne M. Fitzsimons,et al.  Devaluation Versus Enhancement of Attractive Alternatives: A Critical Test Using the Calibration Paradigm , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[51]  E. Kandel,et al.  Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activit , 2006 .

[52]  G. Glover,et al.  Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[53]  John A Bargh,et al.  The unconscious regulation of emotion: nonconscious reappraisal goals modulate emotional reactivity. , 2009, Emotion.

[54]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  E. Finkel,et al.  Self-control and accommodation in close relationships: an interdependence analysis. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  S. Rickless Will and Motivation , 2013 .

[57]  Elliot T. Berkman,et al.  The neural basis of rationalization: cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[58]  Elliot T. Berkman,et al.  Using Neuroscience to Broaden Emotion Regulation: Theoretical and Methodological Considerations. , 2009, Social and personality psychology compass.

[59]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system , 2000, Neuroreport.

[60]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[61]  Jeffry A. Simpson,et al.  Perception of physical attractiveness: Mechanisms involved in the maintenance of romantic relationships. , 1990 .

[62]  John M. Martz,et al.  The Investment Model Scale: Measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size , 1998 .