Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure? Evidence from preschool children's inferences about word meanings.

Four studies examined the relation between children's cognitive inhibition and flexibility in a lexical inference task. Children's linguistic flexibility was assessed by the Flexible Induction of Meaning (FIM) test (Deák, 2000a), which requires that children shift inferences about the meanings of several words for novel objects. In Study 1, 54 3-year-olds either were trained between blocks of problems, for a delay of 3 min, or received no training or delay. Training delays did not influence perseveration. In Study 2 (N=72 3- and 4-year-olds') novel word problems were grouped either to increase the frequency of cue switches (i.e., reduce response "set") or minimize the interval between problems about the same objects. Again, no effect was found. In Study 3, 48 3- and 4-year-olds completed 6 preliminary trials; in a high interference group these trials generated a response set to be inhibited upon the first switch to a new cue context. This group did not perseverate more than a control group. There was no association between FIM perseveration and a Stroop-like test of verbal inhibition though both were marginally related to receptive vocabulary. In study 4 (48 3- and 4-year-olds), FIM was again unrelated to Stroop performance, but was related to the ability to tell whether a situation or problem is indeterminate. Thus, flexibility across semantic inferences is not influenced by timing, order, and number of pre-switch problems and is not predicted by individual differences in a test of verbal inhibition. However previously reported age and individual differences in flexible induction of word meanings are robust and related to vocabulary and logical ability.

[1]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  D. B. Reutener,et al.  Encoding Processes and Release from Proactive Interference in Short-Term Memory of Preschool Children , 1985 .

[3]  A. Diamond,et al.  The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1 2 –7 years old on a stroop- like day-night test , 1994, Cognition.

[4]  P. H. Miller,et al.  Age differences in the capacity demands of a strategy among spontaneously strategic children. , 1991, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  William Epstein,et al.  Perceptual set as an artifact of recency. , 1960 .

[6]  G. Deák,et al.  The dynamics of preschoolers' categorization choices. , 1996, Child development.

[7]  Gedeon O. Deák,et al.  The Growth of Flexible Problem Solving: Preschool Children Use Changing Verbal Cues to Infer Multiple Word Meanings , 2000 .

[8]  J. Speer,et al.  Two Practical Strategies Young Children Use to Interpret Vague Instructions. , 1984 .

[9]  J. Enns The Development of attention : research and theory , 1990 .

[10]  W. Neill,et al.  Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. , 1995 .

[11]  H. Wellman,et al.  Selective information use and perseveration in the search behavior of infants and young children. , 1983, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  C. Patterson,et al.  Plans and the Development of Listener Skills. , 1977 .

[13]  C. Cameron Trials per problem and age as factors in learning set formation of children , 1979 .

[14]  Maxim I. Stamenov,et al.  Language structure, discourse and the access to consciousness , 1997 .

[15]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  The A‐Not‐B Error: Results from a Logistic Meta‐Analysis , 1999 .

[16]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of popout: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection , 2000 .

[17]  A. Luria,et al.  The directive function of speech in development and dissolution , 1959 .

[18]  D. Olson From Utterance to Text: The Bias of Language in Speech and Writing , 1977 .

[19]  G. Deák,et al.  The effects of task comprehension on preschoolers' and adults' categorization choices. , 1995, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

[20]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[21]  Olivier Houdé,et al.  Inhibition and cognitive development: object, number, categorization, and reasoning , 2000 .

[22]  Douglas Frye,et al.  Cognitive complexity and control: A theory of the development of deliberate reasoning and intentional action , 1997 .

[23]  K. K. Harnishfeger,et al.  The development of cognitive inhibition: Theories, definitions, and research evidence , 1995 .

[24]  M. Scheerer,et al.  Problem Solving , 1967, Nature.

[25]  Peter A. Ornstein,et al.  Chapter 16 Attentional Capacity and Children's Memory Strategy Use , 1990 .

[26]  P. Zelazo,et al.  An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them ☆ , 1996 .

[27]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  Interference and inhibition in cognition: An historical perspective , 1995 .

[28]  E. Lorch,et al.  Attentional inertia reduces distractibility during young children's TV viewing. , 1987, Child development.

[29]  G. Fauconnier Mappings in thought and language , 1997 .

[30]  J. Nigg On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  L. R. Peterson,et al.  Short-term retention of individual verbal items. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[33]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Determinants of negative priming. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  Age‐related Asynchrony of Knowledge and Action , 1991 .

[35]  D. Wertlieb,et al.  Maturation of Maze Behavior in Preschool Children. , 1979 .

[36]  G. Deák,et al.  Children's perseverative appearance-reality errors are related to emerging language skills. , 2003, Child development.

[37]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[38]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young children's sensitivity to logical necessity in their inferential search behavior. , 1987, Child development.

[39]  H. Wellman,et al.  Comprehension monitoring in preschool children. , 1985, Child development.

[40]  C. Cameron Interference effects on preschool children's learning sets , 1984 .

[41]  Josef Perner,et al.  Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: why alternative naming comes with understanding belief , 2002 .

[42]  M. Reed,et al.  Inhibitory Self-Control in Preschool Children. , 1984 .

[43]  A. Luchins Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. , 1942 .

[44]  Carla J. Johnson Inhibitory mechanisms in selection among multiple object names , 1994 .

[45]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[46]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. , 1990 .

[47]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging , 1992 .

[48]  P. Zelazo,et al.  Rule selection versus rule execution in preschoolers: an error-detection approach. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[49]  Thomas C. Toppino,et al.  Effect of Perceptual Pretraining on Children's Concept Performance with Nonpreferred Relevant Dimensions: Evidence for the Role of Attentional Strategies. , 1979 .

[50]  S. Olson Assessment of Impulsivity in Preschoolers: Cross-Measure Convergences, Longitudinal Stability, and Relevance to Social Competence , 1989 .

[51]  J. Dixon,et al.  Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 2001 .

[52]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[53]  Bruce F. Pennington,et al.  A normative‐developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children , 1991 .

[54]  Gedeon O Deák,et al.  The development of cognitive flexibility and language abilities. , 2003, Advances in child development and behavior.

[55]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[56]  G. Deák Hunting the Fox of Word Learning: Why "Constraints" Fail To Capture It. , 2000 .

[57]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Sources of interference from irrelevant information: a developmental study. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[58]  L. Mcdonough,et al.  The role of semantic context and memory in the acquisition of novel nouns. , 1998, Child development.

[59]  B. DeSchepper,et al.  An inhibition-based fan effect : Evidence for an active suppression mechanism in selective attention , 1992 .