Effects of aspirin on psychophysical measures of frequency selectivity, two-tone suppression, and growth of masking.

Three psychophysical measures of nonlinearity were evaluated before and during a course of aspirin ingestion to investigate the role of outer hair cells (OHCs) in these measures, as aspirin is thought to alter the functioning of OHCs. Six normal-hearing individuals received a moderate dose (3.9 g/day) of aspirin for four days, producing essentially flat, temporary hearing losses that ranged from 5-20 dB. The losses were about 2 dB greater for a 300-ms signal than for a 15-ms signal, indicating reduced temporal integration with aspirin. On the final three days of aspirin use, three experiments were completed; each was designed to measure one aspect of nonlinear behavior: (1) the effects of level on frequency selectivity in simultaneous masking using notched-noise maskers, (2) two-tone suppression using forward maskers at the signal frequency (fs) and suppressor tones above fs, and (3) growth-of-masking functions in forward masking using a masking tone below fs. Signal frequencies of 750 and 3000 Hz were used to evaluate the effects of aspirin at relatively low- and high-frequency regions of the cochlea. In experiment 1, aspirin broadened the auditory filters and reduced the effect of level on frequency selectivity. In experiment 2, aspirin reduced or eliminated two-tone suppression. And, in experiment 3, aspirin reduced the slopes of the growth-of-masking functions. Thus, the aspirin was effective in reducing nonlinearity in all three experiments, suggesting that these measures reflect the same (or a similar) active, nonlinear mechanism, namely the compressive nonlinearity provided by the OHCs. In all experiments, aspirin tended to have larger detrimental effects on the nonlinear measures at 3000 Hz than at 750 Hz, which can be explained in terms of greater involvement of nonlinear processing at higher frequencies. Finally, these effects of aspirin were found to be similar to those observed in preliminary measurements in two subjects with mild, permanent hearing loss.

[1]  D A Nelson,et al.  High-level psychophysical tuning curves: forward masking in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. , 1991, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[2]  G. Smoorenburg Effects of temporary threshold shift on combination-tone generation and on two-tone suppression , 1980, Hearing Research.

[3]  R. A. Schmiedt Boundaries of two-tone rate suppression of cochlear-nerve activity , 1982, Hearing Research.

[4]  B C Moore,et al.  Characterization and Simulation of Impaired Hearing: Implications for Hearing Aid Design , 1991, Ear and hearing.

[5]  D. McFadden,et al.  Aspirin abolishes spontaneous oto-acoustic emissions. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  D L Neff Stimulus parameters governing confusion effects in forward masking. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  D. McFadden,et al.  Aspirin-induced hearing loss as a model of sensorineural hearing loss , 1984, Hearing Research.

[8]  B C Moore,et al.  Short-term temporal integration: evidence for the influence of peripheral compression. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  M. Sachs,et al.  Two-tone inhibition in auditory-nerve fibers. , 1968, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  A. Oxenham,et al.  A behavioral measure of basilar-membrane nonlinearity in listeners with normal and impaired hearing. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  B C Moore,et al.  Auditory filter shapes in subjects with unilateral and bilateral cochlear impairments. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  Hugo Fastl,et al.  Temporal integration in normal hearing, cochlear impairment, and impairment simulated by masking , 1987 .

[13]  D A Nelson,et al.  Linearized response growth inferred from growth-of-masking slopes in ears with cochlear hearing loss. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  M. A. Bouman,et al.  Relation between Hearing Threshold and Duration for Tone Pulses , 1959 .

[15]  B C Moore,et al.  Contralateral and ipsilateral cueing in forward masking. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  M R Leek,et al.  Auditory filter shapes of normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners in continuous broadband noise. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  P G Stelmachowicz,et al.  Growth of masking as a measure of response growth in hearing-impaired listeners. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  William S. Rhode,et al.  Two-tone suppression and distortion production on the basilar membrane in the hook region of cat and guinea pig cochleae , 1993, Hearing Research.

[19]  S Rosen,et al.  Auditory filter nonlinearity at 2 kHz in normal hearing listeners. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  P. Fitzgibbons,et al.  Gap detection in normal and hearing-impaired listeners. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  Brian R Glasberg,et al.  Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data , 1990, Hearing Research.

[22]  P. A. McCabe,et al.  XXVIII The Effect of Aspirin upon Auditory Sensitivity , 1965 .

[23]  D. McFadden,et al.  Temporary hearing loss induced by combinations of intense sounds and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs. , 1984, American journal of otolaryngology.

[24]  Growth of masking in sensorineural hearing loss. , 1991, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[25]  S P Bacon,et al.  Psychophysical suppression as a function of signal frequency: noise and tonal maskers. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  B C Moore,et al.  Simplified measurement of auditory filter shapes using the notched-noise method. , 1992, British journal of audiology.

[27]  Effects of aspirin on human auditory filters , 1993, Hearing Research.

[28]  D. McFadden,et al.  Reductions in overshoot during aspirin use. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  W. Brownell,et al.  Effects of salicylate on shape, electromotility and membrane characteristics of isolated outer hair cells from guinea pig cochlea. , 1991, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[30]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  J. Bernstein,et al.  SALICYLATE OTOTOXICITY: A CLINICAL STUDY. , 1965, The New England journal of medicine.

[32]  S. Bacon,et al.  Psychophysical measures of auditory nonlinearities as a function of frequency in individuals with normal hearing. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  R. Patterson,et al.  The deterioration of hearing with age: frequency selectivity, the critical ratio, the audiogram, and speech threshold. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  B. Moore,et al.  Growth of forward masking for sinusoidal and noise maskers as a function of signal delay; implications for suppression in noise. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  P. Stypulkowski Mechanisms of salicylate ototoxicity , 1990, Hearing Research.

[36]  B C Moore,et al.  Interactions of masker bandwidth with signal duration and delay in forward masking. , 1981, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  R. Day,et al.  Concentration-response relationships for salicylate-induced ototoxicity in normal volunteers. , 1989, British journal of clinical pharmacology.

[38]  L. Robles,et al.  Two-tone suppression in the basilar membrane of the cochlea: mechanical basis of auditory-nerve rate suppression. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  J J Zwislocki,et al.  Effects of hair cell lesions on responses of cochlear nerve fibers. I. Lesions, tuning curves, two-tone inhibition, and responses to trapezoidal-wave patterns. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  G. Long,et al.  Modification of spontaneous and evoked otoacoustic emissions and associated psychoacoustic microstructure by aspirin consumption. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[41]  D McFadden,et al.  Partial dissociation of spontaneous otoacoustic emissions and distortion products during aspirin use in humans. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[42]  J. Santos-Sacchi,et al.  Effects of Salicylate and Lanthanides on Outer Hair Cell Motility and Associated Gating Charge , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  S Rosen,et al.  Auditory filter bandwidths as a function of level at low frequencies (125 Hz-1 kHz) , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[44]  R. Carlyon,et al.  Effects of aspirin on human psychophysical tuning curves in forward and simultaneous masking , 1996, Hearing Research.

[45]  D. L. Neff,et al.  Confusion effects with sinusoidal and narrow-band noise forward maskers. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[46]  S. P. Bacon,et al.  Overshoot in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[47]  J R Dubno,et al.  Comparison of frequency selectivity and consonant recognition among hearing-impaired and masked normal-hearing listeners. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.