Neural activities associated with emotion recognition observed in men and women

Previous studies have suggested that men and women process emotional stimuli differently. In this study, we examined if there would be any consistency in regions of activation in men and women when processing stimuli portraying happy or sad emotions presented in the form of facial expressions, scenes, and words. A blocked design BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging paradigm was employed to monitor the neural activities of male and female healthy volunteers while they were presented with the experimental stimuli. The imaging data revealed that the right insula and left thalamus were consistently activated for men, but not women, during emotion recognition of all forms of stimuli studied. To further understand the imaging data acquired, we conducted the protocol analysis method to identify the cognitive processes engaged while the men and women were viewing the emotional stimuli and deciding whether they were happy or sad. The findings suggest that men rely on the recall of past emotional experiences to evaluate current emotional experiences. This may explain why the insula, a structure important for self-induced or internally generated recalled emotions, was consistently activated in men while processing emotional stimuli. Our findings suggest possible gender-related neural responses to emotional stimuli.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multisubject fMRI Studies and Conjunction Analyses , 1999, NeuroImage.

[2]  H. Critchley,et al.  Fear Conditioning in Humans The Influence of Awareness and Autonomic Arousal on Functional Neuroanatomy , 2002, Neuron.

[3]  M. Bradley,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Jack L. Lancaster,et al.  A modality‐independent approach to spatial normalization of tomographic images of the human brain , 1995 .

[5]  A. Damasio,et al.  Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  N. Kalin,et al.  Indications and Guidelines for Plasma Tricyclic Antidepressant Concentration Monitoring , 1981, Journal of clinical psychopharmacology.

[7]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Automated labeling of the human brain: A preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward‐transform method , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[8]  N. Kalin,et al.  Neurochemical mechanisms in the affective disorders and neuroendocrine correlates. , 1981, Journal of clinical psychopharmacology.

[9]  M. Raichle,et al.  A Stereotactic Method of Anatomical Localization for Positron Emission Tomography , 1985, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[10]  Joseph B. Sala,et al.  Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  P. Snyder,et al.  Handedness, Sex, Familial Sinistrality Effects on Spatial Tasks , 1993, Cortex.

[12]  Janet Shibley Hyde,et al.  Gender Differences in Human Cognition , 1997 .

[13]  E. Diener,et al.  Sex differences in the recall of affective experiences. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  Nick Hammond,et al.  HUMAN COGNITION , 1986 .

[15]  P. Lang,et al.  International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings (Tech. Rep. No. A-4) , 1999 .

[16]  Charles D. Smith,et al.  Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Investigation of facial recognition memory and happy and sad facial expression perception: an fMRI study , 1998, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[18]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses , 1997, Neuroreport.

[19]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  P. Fox,et al.  Global spatial normalization of human brain using convex hulls. , 1999, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[21]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Emotion Activation Studies in PET and fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[22]  Chetwyn C. H. Chan,et al.  Gender differences in neural correlates of recognition of happy and sad faces in humans assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[23]  P. Snyder,et al.  Handedness, sex, and familial sinistrality effects on spatial tasks. , 1993, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[24]  B. Turetsky,et al.  Sex Differences in Brain Gray and White Matter in Healthy Young Adults: Correlations with Cognitive Performance , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[26]  T. Hendler,et al.  Area-Specific Amblyopic Effects in Human Occipitotemporal Object Representations , 2003, Neuron.

[27]  David P. Friedman,et al.  A modality-specific somatosensory area within the insula of the rhesus monkey , 1993, Brain Research.

[28]  Faith M. Gunning-Dixon,et al.  Sex differences in temporo-limbic and frontal brain volumes of healthy adults. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  D. Perrett,et al.  Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  M. Keshavan,et al.  Sex differences in brain maturation during childhood and adolescence. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Jack L. Lancaster,et al.  Clustered pixels analysis for functional MRI activation studies of the human brain , 1995 .