Excessive Computer Game Playing among Norwegian Adults: Self-Reported Consequences of Playing and Association with Mental Health Problems

Computer games are the most advanced form of gaming. For most people, the playing is an uncomplicated leisure activity; however, for a minority the gaming becomes excessive and is associated with negative consequences. The aim of the present study was to investigate computer game-playing behaviour in the general adult Norwegian population, and to explore mental health problems and self-reported consequences of playing. The survey includes 3,405 adults 16 to 74 years old (Norway 2007, response rate 35.3%). Overall, 65.5% of the respondents reported having ever played computer games (16-29 years, 93.9%; 30-39 years, 85.0%; 40-59 years, 56.2%; 60-74 years, 25.7%). Among 2,170 players, 89.8% reported playing less than 1 hr. as a daily average over the last month, 5.0% played 1-2 hr. daily 3.1% played 2–4 hr. daily, and 2.2% reported playing>4 hr. daily. The strongest risk factor for playing > 4 hr. daily was being an online player, followed by male gender, and single marital status. Reported negative consequences of computer game playing increased strongly with average daily playing time. Furthermore, prevalence of self-reported sleeping problems, depression, suicide ideations, anxiety, obsessions/compulsions, and alcohol/substance abuse increased with increasing playing time. This study showed that adult populations should also be included in research on computer game-playing behaviour and its consequences.

[1]  E. Walker,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[2]  R. Gråwe,et al.  Gambling behavior and gambling problems in Norway 2007. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[3]  Mark D. Griffiths,et al.  Experiences of Time Loss among Videogame Players: An Empirical Study , 2007, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[4]  S. Krokstad,et al.  [How is self-rated health associated with mortality?]. , 2006, Tidsskrift for den Norske laegeforening : tidsskrift for praktisk medicin, ny raekke.

[5]  M. Davies,et al.  EverQuest—It’s Just a Computer Game Right? An Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis of Online Gaming Addiction , 2006, International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction.

[6]  Mark D. Griffiths,et al.  Affect and the Computer Game Player: The Effect of Gender, Personality, and Game Reinforcement Structure on Affective Responses to Computer Game-Play , 2006, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[7]  Mark Griffiths,et al.  Video games and health , 2005, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[8]  Rodney X. Sturdivant,et al.  Applied Logistic Regression: Hosmer/Applied Logistic Regression , 2005 .

[9]  Agneta Johansson,et al.  Problems with Computer Games without Monetary Reward: Similarity to Pathological Gambling , 2004, Psychological reports.

[10]  Mark D. Griffiths,et al.  Demographic Factors and Playing Variables in Online Computer Gaming , 2004, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[11]  M. Davies,et al.  Online computer gaming: a comparison of adolescent and adult gamers. , 2004, Journal of adolescence.

[12]  D. Hodgins,et al.  Trusting problem gamblers: reliability and validity of self-reported gambling behavior. , 2003, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[13]  Ricardo A Tejeiro Salguero,et al.  Measuring problem video game playing in adolescents. , 2002, Addiction.

[14]  E. Peper,et al.  Cumulative Trauma Disorder Risk for Children Using Computer Products: Results of a Pilot Investigation with a Student Convenience Sample , 2002, Public health reports.

[15]  S. Fisher,et al.  Identifying video game addiction in children and adolescents. , 1994, Addictive behaviors.

[16]  I Russell,et al.  Statistics--with confidence? , 1991, The British journal of general practice : the journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners.

[17]  Mark D. Griffiths,et al.  Breaking the Stereotype: The Case of Online Gaming , 2003, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..