Developmental changes and individual differences in risk and perspective taking in adolescence

Abstract Despite the assumed prevalence of risk-taking behavior in adolescence, the laboratory evidence of risk taking remains scarce, and the individual variation poorly understood. Drawing from neuroscience studies, we tested whether risk and reward orientation are influenced by the perspective that adolescents take when making risky decisions. Perspective taking was manipulated by cuing participants prior to each choice whether the decision was made for “self,” or from the perspective of an “other” (the experimenter in Experiment 1; a hypothetical peer in Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, we show a developmental decrease in risk-taking behavior across different stages of adolescence. In addition, all age groups made fewer risky choices for the experimenter, but the difference between self and other was larger in early adolescence. In Experiment 2, we show that high sensation-seeking (SS) adolescents make more risky choices than low SS adolescents, but both groups make a similar differentiation for other individuals (low risk-taking or high risk-taking peers). Together, the results show that younger adolescents and high SS adolescents make more risky choices for themselves, but can appreciate that others may make fewer risky choices. The developmental change toward more rational decisions versus emotional, impulsive decisions may reflect, in part, more efficient integration of others’ perspectives into one's decision making. These developmental results are discussed regarding brain systems important for risk taking and perspective taking.

[1]  E. Crone,et al.  Development of decision making in school-aged children and adolescents: evidence from heart rate and skin conductance analysis. , 2007, Child development.

[2]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  G. Glover,et al.  Reflecting upon Feelings: An fMRI Study of Neural Systems Supporting the Attribution of Emotion to Self and Other , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  L. Steinberg Cognitive and affective development in adolescence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  F. Castellanos,et al.  An fMRI examination of developmental differences in the neural correlates of uncertainty and decision-making. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[7]  R. Barkley,et al.  Executive Functioning, Temporal Discounting, and Sense of Time in Adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[8]  Ty W. Boyer The development of risk-taking: A multi-perspective review , 2006 .

[9]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Adolescent Development , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology.

[10]  E. Crone,et al.  A Developmental Study of Risky Decisions on the Cake Gambling Task: Age and Gender Analyses of Probability Estimation and Reward Evaluation , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[11]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment , 2001, Science.

[14]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation , 2002, Neuron.

[15]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Neural correlates of developmental differences in risk estimation and feedback processing , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  D. Kandel The Parental and Peer Contexts of Adolescent Deviance: An Algebra of Interpersonal Influences , 1996 .

[17]  L. Steinberg Risk Taking in Adolescence: What Changes, and Why? , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  R. Adolphs Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Will M Aklin,et al.  Evaluation of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risk-taking behaviours. , 2003, Journal of adolescence.

[20]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. , 2006 .

[21]  G. Glover,et al.  The Role of Ventral Frontostriatal Circuitry in Reward-Based Learning in Humans , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Incentive-Elicited Brain Activation in Adolescents: Similarities and Differences from Young Adults , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  H J Eysenck,et al.  Sensation seeking in England and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. , 1978, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[24]  Maynard L. Erickson,et al.  Delinquency is still group behavior: Toward revitalizing the group premise in the sociology of deviance. , 1977 .

[25]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[26]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Longitudinal Mapping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal Children , 2022 .

[27]  Michael G. Hardin,et al.  Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence , 2005, Psychological Medicine.

[28]  E. Crone,et al.  Cardiac concomitants of performance monitoring: context dependence and individual differences. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[29]  J. Arnett Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective , 1992 .

[30]  E. Leibenluft,et al.  The social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology , 2005, Psychological Medicine.

[31]  K. Carey,et al.  Peer influences on college drinking: a review of the research. , 2001, Journal of substance abuse.

[32]  P. Zelazo,et al.  Development of Affective Decision Making for Self and Other , 2005, Psychological science.

[33]  C. Moore,et al.  Intentional relations and social understanding , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[34]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of action representation during adolescence , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[36]  Laurence Steinberg,et al.  Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[37]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Decision-making in disinhibited adolescents and adults: Insensitivity to future consequences or driven by immediate reward? , 2003 .

[38]  E. Crone,et al.  Developmental Changes in Real Life Decision Making: Performance on a Gambling Task Previously Shown to Depend on the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[39]  W. Overman,et al.  Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[41]  W. Overman,et al.  Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[42]  M. Ernst,et al.  Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: Development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  F. Castellanos,et al.  Temporal and probabilistic discounting of rewards in children and adolescents: Effects of age and ADHD symptoms , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Jesper Tegnér,et al.  Brain activity related to working memory and distraction in children and adults. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.

[46]  J. Arnett Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[47]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Functional connectivity with anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortices during decision-making. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[48]  Amiram D. Vinokur,et al.  Review and theoretical analysis of the effects of group processes upon individual and group decisions involving risk. , 1971 .

[49]  Catalina J. Hooper,et al.  Adolescents' performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[50]  C. Frith The social brain? , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[51]  G. Glover,et al.  Earlier Development of the Accumbens Relative to Orbitofrontal Cortex Might Underlie Risk-Taking Behavior in Adolescents , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  G. Glover,et al.  Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: who is at risk? , 2007, Developmental science.

[54]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.