Breastfeeding, Long-Chain Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids in Colostrum, and Infant Mental Development

BACKGROUND: Breastfeeding has been associated with improved neurodevelopment in children. However, it remains unknown to what extent nutritional advantages of breast milk may explain this relationship. OBJECTIVE: We assessed the role of parental psychosocial factors and colostrum long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (LC-PUFA) levels in the relationship between breastfeeding and children's neurodevelopment. METHODS: A population-based birth cohort was established in the city of Sabadell (Catalonia, Spain) as part of the INMA-INfancia y Medio Ambiente Project. A total of 657 women were recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy. Information about parental characteristics and breastfeeding was obtained by using a questionnaire, and trained psychologists assessed mental and psychomotor development by using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development in 504 children at 14 months of age. RESULTS: A high percentage of breastfeeds among all milk feeds accumulated during the first 14 months was positively related with child mental development (0.37 points per month of full breastfeeding [95% confidence interval: 0.06–0.67]). Maternal education, social class, and intelligence quotient only partly explained this association. Children with a longer duration of breastfeeding also exposed to higher ratios between n-3 and n-6 PUFAs in colostrum had significantly higher mental scores than children with low breastfeeding duration exposed to low levels. CONCLUSIONS: Greater levels of accumulated breastfeeding during the first year of life were related to higher mental development at 14 months, largely independently from a wide range of parental psychosocial factors. LC-PUFA levels seem to play a beneficial role in children's mental development when breastfeeding levels are high.

[1]  J. Condon,et al.  The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: Development of a self-report questionnaire instrument , 1998 .

[2]  R. Feldman,et al.  Direct and indirect effects of breast milk on the neurobehavioral and cognitive development of premature infants. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[3]  A. Caspi,et al.  Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Kenward,et al.  Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls , 2009, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[5]  Bruce N. Ames,et al.  Is docosahexaenoic acid, an n−3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid, required for development of normal brain function? An overview of evidence from cognitive and behavioral tests in humans and animals , 2005 .

[6]  S. Jacobson,et al.  Breastfeeding and IQ: Evaluation of the socio‐environmental confounders , 2002, Acta paediatrica.

[7]  P. Boyce,et al.  Assessment of postnatal paternal–infant attachment: development of a questionnaire instrument , 2008 .

[8]  L. Grummer-Strawn,et al.  Association of Breastfeeding Intensity and Bottle-Emptying Behaviors at Early Infancy With Infants' Risk for Excess Weight at Late Infancy , 2008, Pediatrics.

[9]  Nicolás Olea,et al.  Cohort Profile: the INMA--INfancia y Medio Ambiente--(Environment and Childhood) Project. , 2012, International journal of epidemiology.

[10]  C. Agostoni,et al.  Polyunsaturated fatty acid concentrations in human hindmilk are stable throughout 12-months of lactation and provide a sustained intake to the infant during exclusive breastfeeding: an Italian study , 2000, British Journal of Nutrition.

[11]  O. Gureje,et al.  The validity of two versions of the GHQ in the WHO study of mental illness in general health care , 1997, Psychological Medicine.

[12]  J. Jacobson,et al.  Breastfeeding Effects on Intelligence Quotient in 4- and 11-Year-Old Children , 1999, Pediatrics.

[13]  M. Mead,et al.  Environews | Focus , 2022 .

[14]  M. Zeegers,et al.  FADS1 FADS2 gene variants modify the association between fish intake and the docosahexaenoic acid proportions in human milk. , 2010, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[15]  John B Carlin,et al.  American Journal of Epidemiology Practice of Epidemiology Strategies for Multiple Imputation in Longitudinal Studies , 2022 .

[16]  B. Ames,et al.  Is docosahexaenoic acid, an n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid, required for development of normal brain function? An overview of evidence from cognitive and behavioral tests in humans and animals. , 2005, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[17]  John M. Davis,et al.  Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): an observational cohort study , 2007, The Lancet.

[18]  T. Trikalinos,et al.  Breastfeeding and maternal and infant health outcomes in developed countries. , 2007, Evidence report/technology assessment.

[19]  E. Regidor,et al.  Socioeconomic patterns in the use of public and private health services and equity in health care , 2008, BMC health services research.

[20]  Denise L. Drane,et al.  A critical evaluation of the evidence on the association between type of infant feeding and cognitive development. , 2000, Paediatric and perinatal epidemiology.

[21]  L. Smithers,et al.  Effect of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation of preterm infants on disease risk and neurodevelopment: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. , 2008, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[22]  J. Concato,et al.  How good is the evidence linking breastfeeding and intelligence? , 2002, Pediatrics.

[23]  S. Innis,et al.  Genetic variants of the FADS1 FADS2 gene cluster are associated with altered (n-6) and (n-3) essential fatty acids in plasma and erythrocyte phospholipids in women during pregnancy and in breast milk during lactation. , 2008, The Journal of nutrition.

[24]  A. Gordon Breast-feeding, breast-milk feeding, and intelligence quotient. , 2000, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[25]  R. Uauy,et al.  The roles of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy, lactation and infancy: review of current knowledge and consensus recommendations , 2008, Journal of perinatal medicine.

[26]  K. Simmer An intervention promoting exclusive and prolonged breast feeding improved verbal intelligence scores in children at 6.5 years , 2008, Evidence-based medicine.

[27]  K. Michaelsen,et al.  Effects of breast-feeding on cognitive function. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[28]  S. Piper,et al.  Use of an Intensity Ratio to Describe Breastfeeding Exclusivity in a National Sample , 2001, Journal of human lactation : official journal of International Lactation Consultant Association.

[29]  C. L. Jensen,et al.  Docosahexaenoic acid and lactation. , 2009, Prostaglandins, leukotrienes, and essential fatty acids.

[30]  T. Karlsson,et al.  Breastfeeding, very long polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA) and IQ at 6 1/2 years of age. , 2004, Acta paediatrica.

[31]  J. Sunyer,et al.  Organochlorine compounds, iodine intake, and thyroid hormone levels during pregnancy. , 2009, Environmental science & technology.

[32]  M. Goldani,et al.  [Breastfeeding and cognitive development]. , 2003, Jornal de pediatria.

[33]  J. Heinrich,et al.  Role of FADS1 and FADS2 polymorphisms in polyunsaturated fatty acid metabolism. , 2010, Metabolism: clinical and experimental.

[34]  Robert W Platt,et al.  Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[35]  James W. Anderson,et al.  Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. , 1999, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[36]  A. Castellote,et al.  Conjugated linoleic acid determination in human milk by fast-gas chromatography. , 2007, Analytica chimica acta.

[37]  A. Castellote,et al.  Differences in fat content and fatty acid proportions among colostrum, transitional, and mature milk from women delivering very preterm, preterm, and term infants. , 2011, Clinical nutrition.

[38]  I. Deary,et al.  Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis , 2006, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[39]  Kristin Saarem,et al.  Maternal supplementation with very-long-chain n-3 fatty acids during pregnancy and lactation augments children's IQ at 4 years of age. , 2003, Pediatrics.

[40]  S. Patole,et al.  Longchain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in preterm infants. , 2004, The Cochrane database of systematic reviews.

[41]  Rosalind J Wright,et al.  Maternal self-esteem, exposure to lead, and child neurodevelopment. , 2008, Neurotoxicology.

[42]  L. Doyle,et al.  Parenting Behavior Is Associated With the Early Neurobehavioral Development of Very Preterm Children , 2009, Pediatrics.

[43]  P. Helms,et al.  Effects of treating postnatal depression on mother-infant interaction and child development , 2007, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[44]  J. Jacobson,et al.  Breast feeding and intelligence in children , 2006, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[45]  M. Kogevinas,et al.  Maternal fish and other seafood intakes during pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at age 4 years , 2009, Public Health Nutrition.

[46]  P. Amiano,et al.  High throughput method for the determination of organochlorine pesticides and polychlorinated biphenyls in human serum. , 2007, Journal of chromatography. B, Analytical technologies in the biomedical and life sciences.