Constructions, Chunking, and Connectionism: The Emergence of Second Language Structure

schema. For a general summary, there are normative descriptions of stages of L2 proficiency that were drawn up in as atheoretical way as possible by the American Council on the Teaching of Foreign Languages (ACTFL) (Higgs, 1984). These Oral Proficiency Guidelines include the following descriptions of novice and intermediate levels that emphasize the contributions of patterns and formulae to the development of later creativity: “Novice Low: Oral production consists of isolated words and perhaps a few highfrequency phrases... Novice High: Able to satisfy partially the requirements of basic communicative exchanges by relying heavily on learned utterances but occasionally expanding these through simple recombinations of their elements... Intermediate: The intermediate level is characterized by an ability to create with the language by combining and recombining learned elements, though primarily in a reactive mode “ (ACTFL, 1986). Constructions, chunking, and connectionism p. 19 Thus the ACTFL repeatedly stresses the constructive potential of collocations and chunks of language. This is impressive because the ACTFL guidelines were simply trying to describe SLA as objectively as possible—there was no initial theoretical focus on formulae -yet nonetheless the role of formulae became readily apparent in the acquisition process. There are several relevant case studies of child SLA. Wong-Fillmore (1976) presented the first extensive longitudinal study that focused on formulaic language in L2 acquisition. Her subject, Nora, acquired and overused a few formulaic expressions of a new structural type during one period, and then amassed a variety of similar forms during the next: previously unanalyzed chunks became the foundations for creative construction (see also Vihman’s, 1980 analyses of her young son Virve’s SLA). Such observations of the formulaic beginnings of child L2 acquisition closely parallel those of Pine and Lieven for L1. There are a few studies which focus on these processes in classroom-based SLA. R. Ellis (1984) described how three classroom learners acquired formulas which allowed them to meet their basic communicative needs in an ESL classroom, and how the particular formulas they acquired reflected input frequency -they were those which more often occurred in the social and organizational contexts that arose in the classroom environment. Weinert (1994) showed how English learners’ early production of complex target-like German FL negation patterns came through the memorization of complex forms in confined linguistic contexts, and that some of these forms were used as a basis for extension of patterns. Myles, Mitchell and Hooper (1998, 1999) describe the first two years of development of interrogatives in a classroom of anglophone French L2 Constructions, chunking, and connectionism p. 20 beginners, longitudinally tracking the breakdown of formulaic chunks such as comment t’appelles-tu?, comment s’appelle-t-il? and òu habites-tu?, the creative construction of new interrogatives by recombination of their parts, and the ways in which formulae fed the constructive process. Bolander (1989) analyzed the role of chunks in the acquisition of inversion in Swedish by Polish, Finnish and Spanish immigrants enrolled in a fourmonth intensive course in Swedish. In Swedish, the inversion of subject-verb after a sentence-initial non-subject is an obligatory rule. Bolander identified the majority of the inversion cases in her data as being of a chunk-like nature with a stereotyped reading such as det kan man säga (that can one say) and det tycker jag (so think I). Inversion in these sort of clauses is also frequent when the object is omitted as in kan man säga (one can say) and tycker jag (think I), and this pattern was also well integrated in the interlanguage of most of these learners. Bolander showed that the high accuracy on these stereotyped initial-object clauses generalized to produce a higher rate of correctness on clauses with non-stereotyped initial objects than was usual for other types of inversion clause in her data, and took this as evidence that creative language was developing out of familiar formulae. Although there are many reviews which discuss the important role of formula use in SLA (e.g., Hakuta 1974; Nattinger & DeCarrico, 1992; Towell & Hawkins, 1994; Weinert, 1995; and Wray, 1992), there is clearly further need for larger-sampled SLA corpora which will allow detailed analysis of acquisition sequences. De Cock (1998) presents analyses of corpora of language-learner productions using automatic recurrent sequence extractions. These show that second language learners use formulae at least as much as native speakers and at times at significantly higher rates. There is much promise Constructions, chunking, and connectionism p. 21 of such computer-based learner corpus studies (Granger, 1998), providing that sufficient trouble is taken to gather the necessarily intensive longitudinal learner data. There is also need to test the predictions of usage-based theories regarding the influences of typeand tokenfrequency as they apply in SLA. 3 Psychological Accounts of Associative Learning This section concerns the psychological learning mechanisms which underpin the acquisition of constructions. Constructivists believe that language is cut of the same cloth as other forms of learning: although it differs importantly from other knowledge in its specific content and problem space, it is acquired using generic learning mechanisms. The Law of Contiguity, the most basic principle of association, pervades all aspects of the mental representation of language: “Objects once experienced together tend to become associated in the imagination, so that when any one of them is thought of, the others are likely to be thought of also, in the same order of sequence or coexistence as before.” (James, 1890, p. 561). 3.1 Chunking What’s the next letter in a sentence beginning ‘T...’? You know it is much more likely to be h, or a vowel, than it is z or other consonants. You know it couldn’t be q. But I’ll warrant you have never been taught this. What is the first word in that sentence? You are likely to plump for the, or that, rather than thinks or theosophy. If ‘The...’, how does it continue? ‘With an adjective or noun’, you might reply. If ‘The cat...’, then what? And then again, complete ‘The cat sat on the...’. Fluent native speakers know a tremendous Constructions, chunking, and connectionism p. 22 amount about the sequences of language at all grains. We know how letters tend to cooccur (common bigrams, trigrams, and other orthographic regularities). We know the phonotactics of our tongue. We know phrase structure regularities. We know thousands of concrete collocations and we know abstract generalizations that derive from them. We have learned to chunk letters, sounds, morphemes, words, phrases, clauses, bits of cooccurring language at all levels. Psycholinguistic experiments show that we are tuned to these regularities in that we process faster and more easily language which accords with the expectations that have come from our unconscious analysis of the serial probabilities in our lifelong history of input (Ellis, in press). We learn these chunks from the very beginnings of learning a second language. Ellis, Lee and Reber (1999) observed people reading their first 64 sentences of a foreign language. While they read, they saw the referent of each sentence, a simple action sequence involving colored geometrical shapes, for example, the sentence ‘miu-ra ko-gi pye-ri lon-da’ was accompanied by a cartoon showing a square moving onto red circles. A linguistic description of this language might include the facts that it is an SOV language, it has adjective-noun word order, obligatory grammatical number (singular/plural) agreement in the form of matching suffix endings of a verb and its subject and of a noun and the adjective that modifies it, that the 64 sentences are all of the type: [N]Subject [A N]Object V, and that lexis was selected from a very small set of 8 words. But such explicit metalinguistic knowledge is not the stuff of early language acquisition. What did the learners make of it? To assess their intake, immediately after seeing each sentence, learners had to repeat as much as they could of it. How did their intake change over time? It gradually improved in all respects. With increasing exposure, Constructions, chunking, and connectionism p. 23 so performance incremented on diverse measures: the proportion of lexis correctly recalled, correct expression of the adjective-noun agreement, correct subject-verb agreement, totally correct sentence, number of correct bigrams and trigrams, and, generally, conformity to the sequential probabilities of the language at letter, word and phrase level. With other measures it was similarly apparent that there was steady acquisition of form-meaning links and of generalisable grammatical knowledge that allowed success on grammaticality judgement tests which were administered later (Ellis et al., 1999). To greater or lesser degree, these patterns, large and small, were being acquired simultaneously and collaboratively. Acquisition of these sequential patterns is amenable to explanation in terms of psychological theories of chunking. The notion of chunking has been at the core of shortterm memory research since Miller (1956) first proposed the term: while the chunk capacity of short-term memory (STM) is fairly constant at 7±2 chunks, its information capacity can be increased by chunking, a useful representational process in that low-level features that co-occur can be organized together and thence referred to as an individual entity. Chunking underlies your superior short-term memory for a patterned phone numbers (e.g. 0800-123777) or letter strings (e.g. AGREEMENTS, or FAMONUBITY) than for a more random sequences (e.g. 4957-632518, CXZDKLWQPM) even though all strings contain the sa

[1]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[2]  M. Gasser Connectionism and Universals of Second Language Acquisition , 1990, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[3]  X. LingCharles Learning the past tense of English verbs , 1994 .

[4]  박근우 The Grammar Book:An ESL/EFL Teacher Course by Marianne Celce-Murcia and Diane Larsen-Freeman Newbury House , 1986 .

[5]  U. Neisser Cognition and reality: principles and implications , 1976 .

[6]  Jean Handscombe,et al.  On TESOL '83: The Question of Control , 1986 .

[7]  Nick C. Ellis,et al.  Rules or associations in the acquisition of morphology ? The frequency by Regularity interaction in human and PDP learning of morphosyntax , 1998 .

[8]  A. Dunn Second time around. , 2016, Nursing times.

[9]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition , 1987, Cogn. Sci..

[10]  J. Reason Actions not as planned : The price of automatisation , 1979 .

[11]  Friedrich Ungerer,et al.  An introduction to cognitive linguistics , 1999 .

[12]  Karin Humphreys,et al.  The psychology of language : from data to theory , 2001 .

[13]  Susan Hunston and David Oakey,et al.  Introducing Applied Linguistics , 2009 .

[14]  D. Biber,et al.  Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English , 1999 .

[15]  D. Berry How implicit is implicit learning , 1997 .

[16]  Adele E. Goldberg,et al.  Learning argument structure generalizations , 2004 .

[17]  D. Larsen-Freeman Chaos/Complexity Science and Second Language Acquisition , 1997 .

[18]  HERBERT A. SIMON,et al.  The Architecture of Complexity , 1991 .

[19]  Michael Lewis The Lexical Approach: The State of ELT and a Way Forward , 2002 .

[20]  J. Pine,et al.  Reanalysing rote-learned phrases: individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[21]  Rod Ellis,et al.  Formulaic Speech in Early Classroom Second Language Development. , 1983 .

[22]  Rod Ellis,et al.  The Study of Second Language Acquisition , 1994 .

[23]  Richard Hudson,et al.  Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume 1. Theoretical prerequisites , 1990 .

[24]  V. Marchman Constraints on Plasticity in a Connectionist Model of the English Past Tense , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  A. P. B. Sardinha Corpus linguistics - investigating language structure and use , 1999 .

[26]  M. Minami How Children Learn the Meanings of Words , 2001 .

[27]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Early syntactic development: A Construction Grammar approach. , 1999 .

[28]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[29]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Sciences of the Artificial , 1970 .

[30]  Nick C. Ellis,et al.  Sequencing in SLA , 1996, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[31]  W. Epstein The influence of syntactical structure of learning. , 1961, The American journal of psychology.

[32]  Nick Chater,et al.  Connectionist and Statistical Approaches to Language Acquisition: A Distributional Perspective. , 1998 .

[33]  G. Miller,et al.  Free recall of redundant strings of letters. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Metaphors We Live by , 1981 .

[35]  George A. Miller,et al.  The science of words , 1991 .

[36]  Alison Wray,et al.  The Focusing Hypothesis: The theory of left hemisphere lateralised language re-examined , 1992 .

[37]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model , 1991, Cognition.

[38]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  Exercises in rethinking innateness , 1997 .

[39]  R. Weinert Some Effects of a Foreign Language Classroom on the Development of German Negation , 1994 .

[40]  U. Neisser Cognition and reality: principles and implications , 1976 .

[41]  J. K. Bock Syntactic persistence in language production , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  Michael Hoey,et al.  Patterns of Lexis In Text , 1991 .

[43]  G. Leech Grammars of spoken English: new outcomes of corpus-oriented research. , 2000 .

[44]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[45]  M. Brent Speech segmentation and word discovery: a computational perspective , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  S Pinker,et al.  Rules of language. , 1991, Science.

[47]  M. Bowerman Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction , 1976 .

[48]  Dan I. Slobin,et al.  The origins of grammaticizable notions: beyond the individual mind , 2014 .

[49]  Richard Hudson,et al.  English word grammar , 1995 .

[50]  A. Goldberg Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure , 1995 .

[51]  R. Langacker Foundations of cognitive grammar , 1983 .

[52]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Ghosts in the mind's machine: Creating and using images in the brain , 1983 .

[53]  Ronald Carter,et al.  Vocabulary: Applied Linguistic Perspectives , 1987 .

[54]  Michael Smith,et al.  Assignment of gender to French nouns in primary and secondary language : a connectionist model , 1992 .

[55]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The emergence of language. , 1999 .

[56]  Judit Kormos,et al.  PROBLEM-SOLVING MECHANISMS IN L2 COMMUNICATION , 1998, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[57]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[58]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Transfer and competition in second language learning , 1992 .

[59]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  The windowing of attention in language , 1995 .

[60]  J. Pine,et al.  Comparing different models of the development of the English verb category , 1998 .

[61]  J. Pine,et al.  Individual differences in early vocabulary development: redefining the referential-expressive distinction , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[62]  Kenneth Ward Church,et al.  Coping with Syntactic Ambiguity or How to Put the Block in the Box on the Table , 1982, CL.

[63]  Eugene Charniak,et al.  Statistical language learning , 1997 .

[64]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  THE ACQUISITION OF CASE MARKING BY ADULT LEARNERS OF RUSSIAN AND GERMAN , 1998, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[65]  Joseph P. Levy,et al.  Connectionist models of memory and language , 1995 .

[66]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Language Acquisition and Use: Learning and Applying Probabilistic Constraints , 1997, Science.

[67]  Harry Bunt,et al.  Advances in Probabilistic and Other Parsing Technologies , 2000 .

[68]  Nick C. Ellis,et al.  Psycholinguistic Determinants of Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning , 1993 .

[69]  S. Soan,et al.  Speech, language and communication needs (SLCN) , 2004 .

[70]  J. Aitchison Words in the mind , 1994 .

[71]  M. Raijmakers Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. , 1997 .

[72]  John Sinclair,et al.  Corpus, Concordance, Collocation , 1991 .

[73]  M. Tomasello First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development , 1994 .

[74]  N. Ellis Vocabulary acquisition: The implicit ins and outs of explicit cognitive mediation. , 1994 .

[75]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Learning to Segment Speech Using Multiple Cues: A Connectionist Model , 1998 .

[76]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Psychological and biological models , 1986 .

[77]  Michael Barlow,et al.  Usage-based models of language , 2000 .

[78]  A. Becker TOWARD A POST‐STRUCTURALIST VIEW OF LANGUAGE LEARNING: A SHORT ESSAY , 1984 .

[79]  R. Weinert The Role of Formulaic Language in Second Language Acquisition: A Review , 1995 .

[80]  Arthur S. Reber,et al.  Representing artificial grammars: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. , 1997 .

[81]  D. Bolinger Meaning and Memory , 1979 .

[82]  Nick C. Ellis,et al.  FREQUENCY EFFECTS IN LANGUAGE PROCESSING , 2002, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[83]  Robert C. Mathews,et al.  Abstractness of implicit knowledge: A cognitive evolutionary perspective. , 1997 .

[84]  Geoffrey Sampson,et al.  A Stochastic Approach to Parsing , 1986, COLING.

[85]  Roger Hawkins,et al.  Approaches to Second Language Acquisition , 1994 .

[86]  Daniel Jurafsky,et al.  A Probabilistic Model of Lexical and Syntactic Access and Disambiguation , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[87]  H. Behrens From construction to construction: Commentary on Michael Tomasello's review essay ’The Return of Constructions‘ on Adele Goldberg's (1995). : ’Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure‘. , 1998 .

[88]  Louise K. Horowitz,et al.  "The Second Time Around" , 2010 .

[89]  S. Pinker,et al.  On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition , 1988, Cognition.

[90]  T. Bever,et al.  The relation between linguistic structure and associative theories of language learning—A constructive critique of some connectionist learning models , 1988, Cognition.

[91]  David Brazil,et al.  口语语法 = A grammar of speech , 1995 .

[92]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind , 1988 .

[93]  H. Gross Errors in Linguistic Performance: Slips of the Tongue, Ear, Pen, and Hand , 1983 .

[94]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  Regular morphology and the lexicon. , 1995 .

[95]  B. MacWhinney Second language acquisition and the competition model , 1997 .

[96]  Maria Bolander,et al.  Bilingualism Across the Lifespan: Prefabs, patterns and rules in interaction? Formulaic speech in adult learners' L2 Swedish , 1989 .

[97]  G. Kempen,et al.  Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: a computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar , 2000, Cognition.

[98]  A. Newell Unified Theories of Cognition , 1990 .

[99]  Roman Taraban,et al.  Gender processing in native and nonnative Russian speakers , 1999, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[100]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Connectionist psycholinguistics: capturing the empirical data , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[101]  N. Schmitt,et al.  Vocabulary in Language Teaching , 2000 .

[102]  Stephen Pit Corder,et al.  Introducing Applied Linguistics , 1975 .

[103]  Martin D. S. Braine,et al.  The Ontogeny of English Phrase Structure: The First Phase , 1963 .

[104]  James E. McDermott,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages. , 1999 .

[105]  Kenneth Ward Church,et al.  PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF A BREADTH-FIRST PARSING ALGORITHM: THEORETICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS , 1987 .

[106]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[107]  S. Narayanan Talking the Talk is Like Walking the Walk : A Computational Model of Verbal Aspect , 1997 .

[108]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Introduction to Connectionist Modelling of Cognitive Processes , 1998 .

[109]  V. Marchman,et al.  U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition , 1991, Cognition.

[110]  Chris Brew,et al.  Stochastic HPSG , 1995, EACL.

[111]  U. Hahn,et al.  German Inflection: Single Route or Dual Route? , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[112]  R. Stevens,et al.  Aspects of Consciousness: Volume 1, Psychological Issues , 1979 .

[113]  Sandra A. Thompson,et al.  Three Frequency Effects in Syntax , 1997 .

[114]  Sylviane Granger,et al.  Learner English on Computer , 1998 .

[115]  A. Reber Implicit learning of artificial grammars , 1967 .

[116]  Garrison W. Cottrell,et al.  Acquiring the Mapping from Meaning to Sounds , 1994, Connect. Sci..

[117]  Garland D. Bills,et al.  Sociolinguistic perspectives on register , 1994 .

[118]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[119]  Rens Bod,et al.  Beyond Grammar: An Experience-Based Theory of Language , 1998 .

[120]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. , 1992 .

[121]  Sylvie De Cock,et al.  A Recurrent Word Combination Approach to the Study of Formulae in the Speech of Native and Non-Native Speakers of English , 1998 .

[122]  Nick C. Ellis Implicit and explicit processes in language acquisition: An introduction. , 1994 .

[123]  N. Ellis Emergentism, Connectionism, and Language Learning. , 1998 .

[124]  A. Beaton,et al.  Factors Affecting the Learning of Foreign Language Vocabulary: Imagery Keyword Mediators and Phonological Short-Term Memory , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[125]  J. Aitchinson The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[126]  D. Hebb Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal , 1961 .

[127]  K. Hakuta PREFABRICATED PATTERNS AND THE EMERGENCE OF STRUCTURE IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION1 , 1974 .

[128]  James R. Nattinger,et al.  Lexical Phrases and Language Teaching , 1992 .

[129]  Nick Chater,et al.  Connectionist models of human language processing : Progress and prospects , 1999 .

[130]  V. Marchman,et al.  From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets , 1993, Cognition.

[131]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Sonderband Connectionist Models of Human Language Processing , 1999 .

[132]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Irresistible environment meets immovable neurons , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[133]  N. Ellis,et al.  Implicit and explicit learning of languages , 1997 .

[134]  菅山 謙正,et al.  Word Grammar 理論の研究 , 2005 .

[135]  F. Myles,et al.  INTERROGATIVE CHUNKS IN FRENCH L2 , 1999, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[136]  Jan Svartvik,et al.  A __ comprehensive grammar of the English language , 1988 .

[137]  Gerry T. M. Altmann,et al.  The Ascent of Babel: An Exploration of Language, Mind, and Understanding , 1997 .

[138]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The Return of Constructions , 1998, Journal of Child Language.

[139]  M. Tomasello Language is not an instinct , 1995 .

[140]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Beyond Rules and Exceptions: A Connectionist Approach to Inflectional Morphology , 1994 .

[141]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The emergence of grammaticality in connectionist networks. , 1999 .

[142]  James R. Nattinger,et al.  A Lexical Phrase Grammar for ESL. , 1980 .

[143]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[144]  J. Pine,et al.  Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category , 1997, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[145]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  SECOND‐LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FROM A FUNCTIONALIST PERSPECTIVE: PRAGMATIC, SEMANTIC, AND PERCEPTUAL STRATEGIES * , 1981 .

[146]  E. Bates,et al.  2 ON THE INSEPARABILITY OF GRAMMAR AND THE LEXICON : EVIDENCE FROM ACQUISITION , APHASIA AND REAL-TIME PROCESSING , 1997 .

[147]  G. A. Miller The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. , 1956, Psychological review.

[148]  Elisabeth Rieken Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective , 2009 .

[149]  F. Myles,et al.  Rote or rule ? Exploring the role of formulaic language in classroom foreign language learning , 1998 .

[150]  Sally Sieloff Magnan,et al.  Teaching for Proficiency, the Organizing Principle , 1985 .

[151]  M. Tomasello The new psychology of language : cognitive and functional approaches to language structure , 1998 .

[152]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Some evidence that irregular forms are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule generated , 1990 .

[153]  Susan Hunston,et al.  Book Reviews: Pattern Grammar: A Corpus-Driven Approach to the Lexical Grammar of English , 2000, CL.

[154]  Nick C. Ellis,et al.  Memory for language , 2001 .