The Perceptual Quality of the Oculus Rift for Immersive Virtual Reality

The recent release of the Oculus Rift, originally developed for entertainment applications, has reignited the interest of researchers and clinicians toward the use of head-mounted-displays in basic behavioral research and physical and psychological rehabilitation. However, careful evaluation of the Oculus Rift is necessary to determine whether it can be effectively used in these novel applications. In this article we address two issues concerning the perceptual quality of the Oculus Rift. (a) Is the Oculus able to generate an acceptable degree of immersivity? In particular, is it possible to elicit the sensation of presence via the virtual stimuli rendered by the device? (b) Does the Virtual Reality experienced through the Oculus Rift induce physical discomfort? To answer these questions, we employed four virtual scenarios in three separate experiments and evaluated performance with objective and subjective outcomes. In Experiment 1 we monitored observers’ heart rate and asked them to rate their Virtual Reality experience via a custom questionnaire. In Experiment 2 we monitored observers’ head movements in reaction to virtual obstacles and asked them to fill out the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (Kennedy et al., 1993) both before and after experiencing Virtual Reality. In Experiment 3 we compared the Oculus Rift against two other low-cost devices used in immersive Virtual Reality: the Google cardboard and a standard 3DTV monitor. Observers’ heart rate increased during exposure to Virtual Reality, and they subjectively reported the experience to be immersive and realistic. We found a strong relationship between observers’ fear of heights and vertigo experienced during one of the virtual scenarios involving heights, suggesting that observers felt a strong sensation of presence within the virtual worlds. Subjects reacted to virtual obstacles by moving to avoid them, suggesting that the obstacles were perceived as real threats. Observers did not experience simulator sickness when the exposure to Virtual Reality was short and did not induce excessive amounts of vection. Compared to the other devices the Oculus Rift elicited a greater degree of immersivity. Thus our investigation suggests that the Oculus Rift head-mounted-display is a potentially powerful tool for a wide array of basic research and clinical applications.

[1]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[2]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  How Immersive Is Enough? A Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Immersive Technology on User Presence , 2016 .

[3]  Victoria Interrante,et al.  Correlations Between Physiological Response, Gait, Personality, and Presence in Immersive Virtual Environments , 2012, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[4]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  The Role of Immersion and Narrative in Mediated Presence: The Virtual Hospital Experience , 2011, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[5]  Marcel P. Lucassen,et al.  Visual comfort of binocular and 3D displays , 2001, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[6]  Gerd Bruder,et al.  A self-experimentation report about long-term use of fully-immersive technology , 2014, SUI.

[7]  Jonathan Freeman,et al.  A Cross-Media Presence Questionnaire: The ITC-Sense of Presence Inventory , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[8]  Chin-Chen Chang,et al.  A PC-based distributed multiple display virtual reality system , 2001 .

[9]  E. Tunik,et al.  Sensorimotor training in virtual reality: a review. , 2009, NeuroRehabilitation.

[10]  Robert S. Kennedy,et al.  Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .

[11]  Mtm Marc Lambooij,et al.  Visual Discomfort and Visual Fatigue of Stereoscopic Displays: A Review , 2009 .

[12]  A. Solimini Are There Side Effects to Watching 3D Movies? A Prospective Crossover Observational Study on Visually Induced Motion Sickness , 2013, PloS one.

[13]  Cristina Botella,et al.  Internet and Virtual Reality as Assessment and Rehabilitation Tools for Clinical Psychology and Neuroscience 3 Virtual Reality and Psychotherapy , 2022 .

[14]  John Powell,et al.  How level of realism influences anxiety in virtual reality environments for a job interview , 2013, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[15]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  On feeling (the) present: An evolutionary account of the sense of presence in physical and electronically-mediated environments , 2010 .

[16]  Regina Bernhaupt,et al.  Introduction to this Special Issue on HCI and Games , 2015, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[17]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  Virtual Interpersonal Touch: Expressing and Recognizing Emotions Through Haptic Devices , 2007, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[18]  Eric J. Seibel,et al.  Feasibility of Articulated Arm Mounted Oculus Rift Virtual Reality Goggles for Adjunctive Pain Control During Occupational Therapy in Pediatric Burn Patients , 2014, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[19]  Mel Slater,et al.  Depth of Presence in Virtual Environments , 1994, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[20]  David M. Hoffman,et al.  Vergence-accommodation conflicts hinder visual performance and cause visual fatigue. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[21]  Carolina Cruz-Neira,et al.  Surround-Screen Projection-Based Virtual Reality: The Design and Implementation of the CAVE , 2023 .

[22]  Simon Davis,et al.  A Systematic Review of Cybersickness , 2014, IE.

[23]  Stéphane Bouchard Could virtual reality be effective in treating children with phobias? , 2011, Expert review of neurotherapeutics.

[24]  Booncharoen Sirinaovakul,et al.  Introduction to the Special Issue , 2002, Comput. Intell..

[25]  Jenny C A Read,et al.  Stereoscopic 3-D content appears relatively veridical when viewed from an oblique angle. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[26]  Gert Pfurtscheller,et al.  Analysis of Physiological Responses to a Social Situation in an Immersive Virtual Environment , 2006, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[27]  Sieu K. Khuu,et al.  The Oculus Rift: a cost-effective tool for studying visual-vestibular interactions in self-motion perception , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[28]  Bernard D. Adelstein,et al.  Demand Characteristics in Assessing Motion Sickness in a Virtual Environment: Or Does Taking a Motion Sickness Questionnaire Make You Sick? , 2007, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[29]  Sumio Yano,et al.  Visual fatigue caused by stereoscopic images and the search for the requirement to prevent them: A review , 2012, Displays.

[30]  Hiromu Ishio,et al.  A Study on Within-Subject Factors for Visually Induced Motion Sickness by Using 8K Display - Through Measurement of Body Sway Induced by Vection While Viewing Images , 2015, HCI.

[31]  Sarah Sharples,et al.  Virtual reality induced symptoms and effects (VRISE): Comparison of head mounted display (HMD), desktop and projection display systems , 2008, Displays.

[32]  Sheng Liu,et al.  A systematic method for designing depth-fused multi-focal plane three-dimensional displays. , 2010, Optics express.

[33]  Mark D. Dunlop,et al.  Toward a Multidisciplinary Model of Context to Support Context-Aware Computing , 2005, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[34]  Mel Slater,et al.  Using Presence Questionnaires in Reality , 2000, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[35]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  Misperceptions in stereoscopic displays: a vision science perspective , 2008, APGV '08.

[36]  Peter A. Howarth,et al.  Visual fatigue caused by viewing stereoscopic motion images: Background, theories, and observations , 2008, Displays.

[37]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  From virtual reality to interreality in the treatment of anxiety disorders , 2011 .

[38]  Christopher D. Saunter,et al.  Dynamic lens and monovision 3D displays to improve viewer comfort , 2015, Optics express.

[39]  Max M. North,et al.  Effectiveness of Virtual Environment Desensitization in the Treatment of Agoraphobia , 1996, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[40]  M. Krijn,et al.  Virtual reality exposure therapy of anxiety disorders: a review. , 2004, Clinical psychology review.

[41]  James Gao,et al.  High-speed switchable lens enables the development of a volumetric stereoscopic display. , 2009, Optics express.

[42]  Kazuhiko Ukai,et al.  Counterroll torsional eye movement in users of head-mounted displays , 2003 .

[43]  Takashi Shibata,et al.  Visual discomfort with stereo displays: effects of viewing distance and direction of vergence-accommodation conflict , 2011, Electronic Imaging.

[44]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Early head movements elicited by visual stimuli or collicular electrical stimulation in the cat , 2001, Vision Research.

[45]  Hiroki Takada,et al.  A Temporal Analysis of Body Sway Caused by Self-Motion During Stereoscopic Viewing , 2015, HCI.