Compensatory coding of body part location in autotopagnosia: Evidence for extrinsic egocentric coding

We report a patient with autotopagnosia (JD) who was unable to code the position of body parts relative to each other and who failed to update the position of body parts after passive movements. JD's performance in Studies 1–4 suggest that her ability to code the dynamic location of body parts with respect to each other ("intrinsic egocentric" spatial coding) was impaired, and that she employed a compensatory strategy by means of which the location of body parts was computed with respect to objects in the environment ("extrinsic egocentric" spatial coding). Studies 5–8 suggest that JD's ability to update hand position information was impaired after passive relative to active movements of her arm. For example, she was impaired in reaching to a target after passive but not active movements of her hand. Taken together, these findings extend previously reported functions of extrinsic egocentric coding to the localisation of body parts and demonstrate a possible dissociation between body part localisation dependent upon proprioceptive and efference copy information.

[1]  B Bioulac,et al.  A quantitative study of neuronal discharge in areas 5, 2, and 4 of the monkey during fast arm movements. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  C. Colby,et al.  Heterogeneity of extrastriate visual areas and multiple parietal areas in the Macaque monkey , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  R Caminiti,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Parietal cortex and movement I. Movement selection and reaching , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Representation of visuomotor space in the parietal lobe of the monkey. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[7]  J. Seal,et al.  Activity of neurons in area 5 during a simple arm movement in monkeys before and after deafferentation of the trained limb , 1982, Brain Research.

[8]  G. Holmes,et al.  Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions , 1911 .

[9]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Coordinate transformations in the representation of spatial information , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[10]  J. Stein The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[11]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[12]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing. Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[15]  I. T. Draper THE ASSESSMENT OF APHASIA AND RELATED DISORDERS , 1973 .

[16]  R. Nass,et al.  The assessment of aphasia and related disorders By Harold Goodglass and edith kaplan philadelphia, lea & febiger, 1983 illustrated, $27.50 (package) , 1984 .

[17]  H. Goodglass,et al.  Disorders in executive control functions among aphasic and other brain-damaged patients. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[18]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: congruent visual and somatic response properties. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  A. Pick,et al.  Störung der Orientierung am eigenen Körper , 1922 .

[20]  Pamela Davis,et al.  Body image , 2007, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[21]  C. Colby Action-Oriented Spatial Reference Frames in Cortex , 1998, Neuron.

[22]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. II. Spatial properties. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  The Role of the Dynamic Body Schema in Praxis: Evidence from Primary Progressive Apraxia , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[25]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Representing spatial information for limb movement: role of area 5 in the monkey. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Stephen M. Williams,et al.  Handedness inventories: Edinburgh versus Annett. , 1991 .

[27]  C. Gross,et al.  The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons , 1995 .

[28]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  The Blackwell dictionary of Cognitive Psychology , 1990 .

[29]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Evidence for a Disturbance of the Body Schema in Neglect , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[30]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  J. Paillard Brain and space , 1991 .

[32]  Laurel J. Buxbaum,et al.  Specialised structural descriptions for human body parts: Evidence from autotopagnosia , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[33]  S Chieffi,et al.  Hand-centred coding of target location in visuo-spatial working memory , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Laurel J Buxbaum,et al.  SPATIO-MOTOR REPRESENTATIONS IN REACHING: EVIDENCE FOR SUBTYPES OF OPTIC ATAXIA. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[35]  Myrna F. Schwartz,et al.  Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: Knowing ‘what for’ but not ‘how’ , 2000 .

[36]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.