Human Heading Estimation During Visually Simulated Curvilinear Motion

Recent studies have suggested that humans cannot estimate their direction of forward translation (heading) from the resulting retinal motion (flow field) alone when rotation rates are higher than approximately 1 deg/sec. It has been argued that either oculomotor or static depth cues are necessary to disambiguate the rotational and translational components of the flow field and, thus, to support accurate heading estimation. We have re-examined this issue using visually simulated motion along a curved path towards a layout of random points as the stimulus. Our data show that, in this curvilinear motion paradigm, five of six observers could estimate their heading relatively accurately and precisely (error and uncertainty < approximately 4 deg), even for rotation rates as high as 16 deg/sec, without the benefit of either oculomotor or static depth cues signaling rotation rate. Such performance is inconsistent with models of human self-motion estimation that require rotation information from sources other than the flow field to cancel the rotational flow.

[1]  Greg L. Zacharias,et al.  A visual cueing model for terrain-following applications , 1983 .

[2]  M. Braunstein,et al.  Induced self-motion in central vision. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  W. Warren,et al.  The role of central and peripheral vision in perceiving the direction of self-motion , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  J. Lackner,et al.  Induction of illusory self-rotation and nystagmus by a rotating sound-field. , 1977, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[5]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  V Henn,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction in motion perception and the generation of nystagmus. , 1980, Neurosciences Research Program bulletin.

[7]  D. M. Green,et al.  Sound localization by human listeners. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[8]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during eye movements , 1994, Vision Research.

[11]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The role of disparity-sensitive cortical neurons in signalling the direction of self-motion , 1990, Nature.

[12]  James A. Crowell,et al.  The perception of heading during eye movements , 1992, Nature.

[13]  Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al.  Visual navigation with a neural network , 1991, Neural Networks.

[14]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.

[15]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[17]  J. Perrone,et al.  A model of self-motion estimation within primate extrastriate visual cortex , 1994, Vision Research.

[18]  R G Erickson,et al.  Vestibular Input to Visual‐Tracking Neurons in Area MST of Awake Rhesus Monkeys , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[19]  D Regan,et al.  Visually guided locomotion: psychophysical evidence for a neural mechanism sensitive to flow patterns. , 1979, Science.

[20]  W. Warren,et al.  Perception of translational heading from optical flow. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. I. Resting discharge and response to constant angular accelerations. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  James A. Crowell,et al.  The effect of depth information on heading judgments , 1996 .

[23]  J A Perrone,et al.  Model for the computation of self-motion in biological systems. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[24]  G. Orban,et al.  Responses of macaque STS neurons to optic flow components: a comparison of areas MT and MST. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R. Andersen,et al.  Head position signals used by parietal neurons to encode locations of visual stimuli , 1995, Nature.

[26]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[27]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Underlying mechanisms of the response specificity of expansion/contraction and rotation cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  E. S. Calvert,et al.  Visual Judgments in Motion , 1954, Journal of Navigation.

[29]  K. Kawano,et al.  Response properties of neurons in posterior parietal cortex of monkey during visual-vestibular stimulation. I. Visual tracking neurons. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  M. Graziano,et al.  Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  H W Leibowitz,et al.  The effects of fixation and restricted visual field on vection-induced motion sickness. , 1990, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[32]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[33]  K. R. Llewellyn,et al.  Visual guidance of locomotion. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  R. B. Post Circular Vection is Independent of Stimulus Eccentricity , 1988, Perception.

[35]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Visual responses of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar flocculus during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. I. Simple spikes. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements , 1996, Vision Research.

[37]  D J Hannon,et al.  Eye movements and optical flow. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[38]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Perception with an eye for motion , 1986 .

[39]  John A. Perrone,et al.  A Role for MST Neurons in Heading Estimation , 1994 .

[40]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. II. Mechanisms of response selectivity revealed by small-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  K. Prazdny,et al.  Determining The Instantaneous Direction Of Motion From Optical Flow Generated By A Curvilinearly Moving Observer , 1981, Other Conferences.

[42]  A. V. D. Berg,et al.  Robustness of perception of heading from optic flow , 1992, Vision Research.

[43]  B J Frost,et al.  Factors affecting the onset and magnitude of linear vection , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  A. Verri,et al.  Mathematical properties of the two-dimensional motion field: from singular points to motion parameters , 1989 .

[45]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading , 1994, Nature.

[46]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III. Response dynamics. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  J H Rieger,et al.  Processing differential image motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[48]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  Analysis of misperceived observer motion during simulated eye rotations , 1994, Vision Research.

[49]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[50]  Leland S. Stone,et al.  Translation and Rotation Trade Off in Human Visual Heading Estimation , 1996 .

[51]  田中 啓治 Analysis of Local and Wide-Field Movements in the Superior Temporal Visual Areas of the Macaque Monkey , 1987 .

[52]  K. Prazdny Determining The Instantaneous Direction Of Motion From Optical Flow Generated By A Curvilinearly Moving Observer , 1981, Other Conferences.

[53]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Response of monkey MST neurons to optic flow stimuli with shifted centers of motion , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  A. W. Blackwell,et al.  Perception of circular heading from optical flow. , 1991 .

[55]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.

[57]  Andrea J. van Doorn,et al.  Invariant Properties of the Motion Parallax Field due to the Movement of Rigid Bodies Relative to an Observer , 1975 .

[58]  J H Rieger,et al.  Information in optical flows induced by curved paths of observation. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[59]  Ellen C. Hildreth,et al.  Recovering heading for visually-guided navigation , 1992, Vision Research.

[60]  Markus Lappe,et al.  A Neural Network for the Processing of Optic Flow from Ego-Motion in Man and Higher Mammals , 1993, Neural Computation.

[61]  Paul A. Braren,et al.  Wayfinding on foot from information in retinal, not optical, flow. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[62]  Kathleen Turano,et al.  Visual discrimination between a curved and straight path of self motion: Effects of forward speed , 1994, Vision Research.

[63]  C. Bonnet,et al.  Properties of curvilinear vection , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[64]  R. Warren The perception of egomotion. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  A. V. D. Berg,et al.  Perception of heading , 1993, Nature.

[66]  J E Cutting,et al.  Wayfinding, displacements, and mental maps: velocity fields are not typically used to determine one's aimpoint. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[67]  A. Watson,et al.  The optimal motion stimulus , 1995, Vision Research.

[68]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[69]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. II. Differentiation of retinal from extraretinal inputs. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  T. Mergner,et al.  Interaction of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs for human self-motion perception , 1992, Neuroscience Letters.

[71]  A. Verri,et al.  First-order analysis of optical flow in monkey brain. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[72]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[74]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[75]  R W Cumming,et al.  The role of optical expansion patterns in locomotor control. , 1973, The American journal of psychology.

[76]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.