Stereo transparency and the disparity gradient limit

Several studies (Vision Research 15 (1975) 583; Perception 9 (1980) 671) have shown that binocular fusion is limited by the disparity gradient (disparity/distance) separating image points, rather than by their absolute disparity values. Points separated by a gradient >1 appear diplopic. These results are sometimes interpreted as a constraint on human stereo matching, rather than a constraint on fusion. Here we have used psychophysical measurements on stereo transparency to show that human stereo matching is not constrained by a gradient of 1. We created transparent surfaces composed of many pairs of dots, in which each member of a pair was assigned a disparity equal and opposite to the disparity of the other member. For example, each pair could be composed of one dot with a crossed disparity of 6' and the other with uncrossed disparity of 6', vertically separated by a parametrically varied distance. When the vertical separation between the paired dots was small, the disparity gradient for each pair was very steep. Nevertheless, these opponent-disparity dot pairs produced a striking appearance of two transparent surfaces for disparity gradients ranging between 0.5 and 3. The apparent depth separating the two transparent planes was correctly matched to an equivalent disparity defined by two opaque surfaces. A test target presented between the two transparent planes was easily detected, indicating robust segregation of the disparities associated with the paired dots into two transparent surfaces with few mismatches in the target plane. Our simulations using the Tsai-Victor model show that the response profiles produced by scaled disparity-energy mechanisms can account for many of our results on the transparency generated by steep gradients.

[1]  A. Parker,et al.  Stereo, surfaces and shape , 1991 .

[2]  David Mumford,et al.  Statistics of range images , 2000, Proceedings IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition. CVPR 2000 (Cat. No.PR00662).

[3]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Is stereopsis effective in breaking camouflage for moving targets? , 1997, Vision Research.

[4]  C. Blakemore The range and scope of binocular depth discrimination in man , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  T. Papathomas Early vision and beyond , 1995 .

[6]  D. Weinshall Seeing “ghost” planes in stereo vision , 1991, Vision Research.

[7]  John P. Frisby,et al.  Transparency and the uniqueness constraint in human and computer stereo vision , 1990, Nature.

[8]  J T Todd,et al.  The perception of stereoscopic transparency , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  A. Parker,et al.  Binocular Neurons in V1 of Awake Monkeys Are Selective for Absolute, Not Relative, Disparity , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[11]  B JULESZ,et al.  Binocular Depth Perception without Familiarity Cues , 1964, Science.

[12]  A. Parker,et al.  The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  W Richards,et al.  Interhemispheric processing of binocular disparity. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  David J. Fleet,et al.  Neural encoding of binocular disparity: Energy models, position shifts and phase shifts , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  Daphna Weinshall,et al.  Perception of multiple transparent planes in stereo vision , 1989, Nature.

[16]  J P Frisby,et al.  PMF: A Stereo Correspondence Algorithm Using a Disparity Gradient Limit , 1985, Perception.

[17]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Encoding of binocular disparity by simple cells in the cat's visual cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  D. Weinshall The computation of multiple matching doubly ambiguous stereograms with transparent planes. , 1993, Spatial vision.

[19]  Peter Lennie,et al.  Spatio-temporal requirements for binocular correlation in stereopsis , 1996, Vision Research.

[20]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Computational models of visual processing , 1991 .

[21]  Scott B. Stevenson,et al.  Human stereo matching is not restricted to epipolar lines , 1997, Vision Research.

[22]  A. Parker,et al.  Range and mechanism of encoding of horizontal disparity in macaque V1. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Encoding of binocular disparity by complex cells in the cat's visual cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[25]  C. Tyler Spatial organization of binocular disparity sensitivity , 1975, Vision Research.

[26]  J. Lappin,et al.  Detection of visual forms in space and time , 1983, Vision Research.

[27]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Qualitative depth localization with diplopic images. , 1956, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[28]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. , 1990, Science.

[29]  G. Henning,et al.  Binocular masking level differences in sinusoidal grating detection. , 1973, Vision research.

[30]  G C DeAngelis,et al.  The physiology of stereopsis. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  M S Gray,et al.  Reliable disparity estimation through selective integration , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[32]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Computational Issues in Solving the Stereo Correspondence ProblemComputational Issues in Solving the Stereo Correspondence Problem , 1991 .

[33]  Ning Qian,et al.  Physiological computation of binocular disparity , 1997, Vision Research.

[34]  B. Julesz,et al.  Modifications of the Classical Notion of Panum's Fusional Area , 1980, Perception.

[35]  Mark Edwards,et al.  Spatial interactions minimize relative disparity between adjacent surfaces , 2001, Vision Research.

[36]  C. Tyler Stereoscopic Vision: Cortical Limitations and a Disparity Scaling Effect , 1973, Science.

[37]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Cooperative computation of stereo disparity , 1988 .

[38]  Ning Qian,et al.  Computing Stereo Disparity and Motion with Known Binocular Cell Properties , 1994, Neural Computation.

[39]  K. Prazdny,et al.  Detection of binocular disparities , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[40]  W. Uttal,et al.  On the detectability of straight lines in visual noise: An extension of French’s paradigm into the millisecond domain , 1970 .

[41]  H. Smallman,et al.  Size-disparity correlation in stereopsis at contrast threshold. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[42]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[43]  Lawrence K. Cormack,et al.  Hyperacuity, superresolution and gap resolution in human stereopsis , 1989, Vision Research.

[44]  Jitendra Malik,et al.  Review of computational models of stereopsis , 1995 .

[45]  B. G. Hertz,et al.  The influence of bandwidth and temporal properties of spatial noise on binocular masking-level differences , 1977, Vision Research.