Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: measuring recollection

How can conscious and unconscious influences of memory be measured? In this article, a processdissociation procedure (L. L. Jacoby, 1991) was used to separate automatic (unconscious) and consciously controlled influences within a task. For recall cued with word stems, automatic influences of memory (a) remained invariant across manipulations of attention that substantially reduced conscious recollection and (b) were highly dependent on perceptual similarity from study to test. Comparisons with results obtained through an indirect test show the advantages of the process-dissociation procedure as a means of measuring unconscious influences. The measure of recollection derived from this procedure is superior to measures gained from classic test theory and signal-detection theory. The process-dissociation procedure combines assumptions from these 2 traditional approaches to measuring memory. Dissociations between performance on direct and indirect tests of memory supply examples of effects of the past in the absence of remembering (for reviews, see Hintzman, 1990; Richardson-Klavehn & Bjork, 1988). In an indirect test, subjects are not asked to report on memory for an event as they would be in a direct test, such as in a test of recognition or recall; rather, they engage in some task that can indirectly reflect memory for the occurrence of that event. Word stemand fragment-completion tasks are among the most popular indirect tests of memory (e.g., Graf & Mandler, 1984; Tulving, Schacter & Stark, 1982; Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1974). For a stem-completion task, subjects might read the word scalp and then be presented with the stem sea— with instructions to complete that stem with the first word that comes to mind. Evidence of automatic influences of memory that are dissociated from performance on a direct test is provided by the finding that prior presentation of a word increases the likelihood of that word being used to complete a stem, even though a direct test reveals no memory for the prior presentation of the word. Some of the most striking examples of dissociations come from the performance of patients suffering a neurological deficit. Korsakoff amnesics, for example, show near-normal effects of memory in their performance of a stem-completion task, even though

[1]  L. Jacoby Remembering the data: analyzing interactive processes in reading , 1983 .

[2]  L. Squire,et al.  Memory: brain systems and behavior , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Attention : selection, awareness, and control : a tribute to Donald Broadbent , 1996 .

[4]  Riccardo Russo,et al.  On the differential nature of implicit and explicit memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[5]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[6]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  The neuropsychology of memory and aging. , 1992 .

[7]  D. L. Hintzman Human learning and memory: connections and dissociations. , 1990, Annual review of psychology.

[8]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  Processing implicit and explicit representations. , 1992, Psychological review.

[9]  C Chiarello,et al.  Adult age differences in implicit and explicit memory: time course and encoding effects. , 1988, Psychology and aging.

[10]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Reinstating study context produces unconscious influences of memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[11]  L. Brooks,et al.  Nonanalytic Cognition: Memory, Perception, and Concept Learning , 1984 .

[12]  D Milech,et al.  Common and modality-specific processes in the mental lexicon , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[13]  A P Shimamura,et al.  Priming Effects in Amnesia: Evidence for a Dissociable Memory Function , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[14]  H L Roediger,et al.  The properties of retrieval cues constrain the picture superiority effect , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[15]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Becoming famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention , 1989 .

[16]  H L Roediger,et al.  Implicit memory. Retention without remembering. , 1990, The American psychologist.

[17]  Gregory V. Jones Independence and Exclusivity Among Psychological Processes: Implications for the Structure of Recall. , 1987 .

[18]  G Mandler,et al.  The information that amnesic patients do not forget. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  E. Eich,et al.  Memory for unattended events: Remembering with and without awareness , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[20]  R. Reed Hunt,et al.  Effect of generation on a word-identification task , 1990 .

[21]  Odmar Neumann,et al.  Automatic Processing: A Review of Recent Findings and a Plea for an Old Theory , 1984 .

[22]  S. Grand,et al.  Recovery in the absence of recall: an investigation of color-word interference. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  M. Masson,et al.  Enhanced perceptual identification without prior perception , 1989 .

[24]  N. J. Slamecka,et al.  The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon , 1978 .

[25]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Unconscious influences of memory: Dissociations and automaticity. , 1992 .

[26]  E. Reingold,et al.  Theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes , 1990 .

[27]  F. Craik,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 1992 .

[28]  Stephan Lewandowsky,et al.  Relating Theory and Data : Essays on Human Memory in Honor of Bennet B. Murdock , 1991 .

[29]  D. Schacter Implicit memory: History and current status. , 1987 .

[30]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Toward a generate/recognize model of performance on direct and indirect tests of memory ☆ , 1990 .

[31]  D. J. Schneider,et al.  Mental control: The war of the ghosts in the machine. , 1995 .

[32]  S. Guttenplan Mind and language , 1975 .

[33]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[34]  Kelly Feenan,et al.  Conceptual priming in fragment completion , 1990 .

[35]  E. Warrington,et al.  The effect of prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Redefining automaticity: Unconscious influences, awareness, and control , 1993 .

[37]  Automaticity and Memory , 1991 .

[38]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  H. Roediger,et al.  Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account , 1989 .

[40]  J SWETS,et al.  Decision processes in perception. , 1961, Psychological review.

[41]  L. Jacoby,et al.  On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  L L Light,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  W. S. Elsbree,et al.  The teachers' handbook , 1929 .

[44]  A P Shimamura,et al.  Priming across modalities and priming across category levels: extending the domain of preserved function in amnesia. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  G. Mandler,et al.  Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. , 1984 .

[46]  A. Milner,et al.  Neuropsychology of Consciousness , 2019, Frontiers Research Topics.

[47]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Measures of Memory , 1988 .

[48]  A. Parkin,et al.  Attention and recollective experience in recognition memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[49]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. , 1982 .

[50]  A. Koriat,et al.  The recovery of incidentally acquired information , 1976 .

[51]  E Tulving,et al.  Priming and human memory systems. , 1990, Science.

[52]  W. Kintsch Learning, memory, and conceptual processes , 1970 .

[53]  Bradford H. Challis,et al.  Level of processing affects priming in word fragment completion. , 1992 .

[54]  Mieke Verfaellie,et al.  Fluency versus conscious recollection in the word completion performance of amnesic patients , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[56]  S. Corkin,et al.  Intact priming of patterns despite impaired memory , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Reenacting the route to interpretation: Enhanced perceptual identification without prior perception. , 1992 .

[58]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Unconscious perception: attention, awareness, and control. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  R. B. Freeman,et al.  Cognition and Motor Processes , 1983 .